Open Access
Article  |   June 2017
Visual wetness perception based on image color statistics
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision June 2017, Vol.17, 7. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/17.5.7
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Masataka Sawayama, Edward H. Adelson, Shin'ya Nishida; Visual wetness perception based on image color statistics. Journal of Vision 2017;17(5):7. https://doi.org/10.1167/17.5.7.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Color vision provides humans and animals with the abilities to discriminate colors based on the wavelength composition of light and to determine the location and identity of objects of interest in cluttered scenes (e.g., ripe fruit among foliage). However, we argue that color vision can inform us about much more than color alone. Since a trichromatic image carries more information about the optical properties of a scene than a monochromatic image does, color can help us recognize complex material qualities. Here we show that human vision uses color statistics of an image for the perception of an ecologically important surface condition (i.e., wetness). Psychophysical experiments showed that overall enhancement of chromatic saturation, combined with a luminance tone change that increases the darkness and glossiness of the image, tended to make dry scenes look wetter. Theoretical analysis along with image analysis of real objects indicated that our image transformation, which we call the wetness enhancing transformation, is consistent with actual optical changes produced by surface wetting. Furthermore, we found that the wetness enhancing transformation operator was more effective for the images with many colors (large hue entropy) than for those with few colors (small hue entropy). The hue entropy may be used to separate surface wetness from other surface states having similar optical properties. While surface wetness and surface color might seem to be independent, there are higher order color statistics that can influence wetness judgments, in accord with the ecological statistics. The present findings indicate that the visual system uses color image statistics in an elegant way to help estimate the complex physical status of a scene.

Introduction
The ability to discriminate and identify color is fundamental to human vision. The evolutionary importance of color is often stated in terms of color identification, such as the ability to judge ripeness of fruit (Mollon, 1989; Steward & Cole, 1989). The issues surrounding color discrimination and identification, including metamerism and color constancy, have been central concerns of color science (Wyszecki & Stiles, 1982; Arend & Reeves, 1986; Maloney & Wandell, 1986; Brainard & Freeman, 1997; Zaidi, Spehar, & DeBonet, 1997; Kraft & Brainard, 1999). 
A surface has many important attributes beyond color. One of these is whether it is wet or dry, a question that is important when eating a piece food, grasping a handrail, or taking a step. Since water is colorless, one might not expect trichromatic vision to be particularly helpful in estimating a surface's wetness. However, we show here that color statistics can be informative even when color identification is not at issue. In particular, when a surface becomes wet, the colors tend to become more saturated. It is straightforward to show, both theoretically and empirically, that this shift in statistics occurs at the physical level, through the interaction of light and matter. It then becomes interesting to ask whether the human visual system “knows” about this shift, and uses it in estimating wetness. 
Many a child has gathered attractive wet pebbles along the beach, only to find that their colors fade when they are dry. Figure 1a shows a beach pebble, with one half wet and one half dry. It is easy to see which side is wet: The wet side looks darker and is adorned with bright specular highlights. Less obvious is the fact that the color statistics have changed. The colors are generally more saturated. 
Figure 1
 
Optical properties of a surface largely change before and after wetting. (a) Demonstration of the change in the appearance of a surface by wetting. (b–e) Schematic diagrams of optical properties of wet and dry surfaces. (b) Light paths on a rough-surfaced solid material (dry). (c) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water. (d) Light paths on a porous material (dry). (e) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water.
Figure 1
 
Optical properties of a surface largely change before and after wetting. (a) Demonstration of the change in the appearance of a surface by wetting. (b–e) Schematic diagrams of optical properties of wet and dry surfaces. (b) Light paths on a rough-surfaced solid material (dry). (c) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water. (d) Light paths on a porous material (dry). (e) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water.
The change in color statistics with wetness is the main topic of this paper. We discuss the physical processes that lead to these changes, and show how modifying image statistics with digital manipulation can alter the perception of a surface's wetness. In addition to changing color statistics, wetness tends to increase the skew of the luminance distribution, which has been suggested to make surfaces apparently darker, glossier (Motoyoshi, Nishida, Sharan, & Adelson, 2007), and fresher (Arce-Lopera, Masuda, Kimura, Wada, & Okajima, 2012). We can bundle the color and luminance cues together to form what we call the wetness enhancing transformation (WET) that can mimic the effects of wetness. We show that the WET operator tends to increase the appearance of wetness when applied to images of a variety of surfaces. The WET operator is not all-powerful: It works well on some images, and poorly on others. By studying the conditions for its success and failure, we can better understand how color information influences wetness perception. 
Optical aspects of wet and dry materials
Among many visual effects caused by object wetting (e.g., see Rungjiratananon, Kanamori, & Nishita, 2012, for detailed computer simulation of the appearance of wet hairs), the present study focuses on optical effects. 
The refractive index of air is approximately 1, while that of most common dielectric (nonmetal) solids (such as plastic, wood, ceramics, and most rocks) is between 1.4 and 1.8. When light strikes a dielectric surface, some of the light is refracted and enters the material, while some undergoes Fresnel reflection at the surface. The amount of reflection depends on the polarization (which we will ignore), the angle of incidence, and the relative refractive indices. The refractive index of water is about 1.33, which is lower than most dielectrics but higher than air. When water is in contact with a solid surface, it offers partial index matching. The amount of reflection is reduced, and the strength of refraction is reduced. Thus, when a light ray enters a solid from water, it is less likely to be reflected, and will be less strongly refracted. The waterborne rays have a better chance of penetrating the surface and traveling within it. 
Consider the case of a rough-surfaced solid material consisting of a colored medium filled with scattering particles, as shown in Figure 1b. A few light paths are shown. (Note: In these illustrations we only show rays that escape back to the air, since these are the ones that can potentially be seen, but of course there are many paths, not shown, that end in absorption.) The simplest thing that can happen is a single reflection at the air interface, shown by Path 1. Double reflections can also occur, as in Path 2. Since the surface is rough, the reflection may happen in any number of directions. In the aggregate the reflections can be considered to be in random directions, and from a modest distance the rough surface acts as a diffuse reflector. Fresnel reflection tends to be chromatically neutral, so the rough surface provides a diffuse neutral haze that is added to any light returning after penetrating the surface. Path 3 shows a ray that penetrates into the solid medium and is scattered twice before re-emerging. Since the medium is colored, some wavelengths will be more attenuated than others during this process, and the emerging ray will therefore be colored. A ray that travels a larger distance within the material will undergo more absorption and so will be dimmer and more saturated in color. 
Figure 1c shows the same material after wetting with water. The water forms a film of some thickness. Because of surface tension, the water's surface is smoother than that of the material. This means that the Fresnel reflections from the water (Path 4) will tend to appear as sharp highlights rather than as a diffuse haze (Jensen, Legakis, & Dorsey, 1999). Path 5 shows a ray that passes into the water and then penetrates a short distance into the solid before being scattered out. The water's partial index matching enhances the ray's tendency to be refracted (rather than reflected) on the way in as well as on the way out of the solid. Path 6 shows a case involving total internal reflection at the air–water interface (Lekner & Dorf, 1988; Lu, Georghiades, Rushmeier, Dorsey, & Xu, 2006). This ray enters and exits the solid medium, is reflected from the water's inner surface, and enters the solid a second time before exiting into the water and the air. This kind of ray can traverse a good distance inside the solid, and can become strongly colored. 
It is impossible to enumerate all the paths that light can take in this situation, since there are so many possible combinations of reflection, refraction, and absorption. However, wetting the surface changes distributions of the various kinds of paths. 
Another kind of material is porous and consists of finely divided particles (or fibers) with fine air spaces. Examples would be a porous rock like sandstone, and fibrous materials such as paper or fabric. In the dry state, these materials offer a plethora of air-dielectric interfaces, which in turn offer many opportunities for Fresnel reflections. This is shown in Figure 1d. Path 7 is reflected directly from a particle. Path 8 undergoes two Fresnel reflections, both of them at air interfaces. In Path 9, the light enters a particle but cannot travel a great distance before exiting. 
Figure 1e shows some paths from a porous material that has become saturated with water. The water offers partial index matching it its interfaces with all the particles. It also provides a smooth surface that replaces the diffusely reflecting random surface of the dry material. Path 10 shows specular reflection at the air–water interface. Path 11 shows a ray that penetrates the water and then, due to partial index matching, is able to travel through multiple particles before exiting (Twomey, Bohren, & Mergenthaler, 1986). 
Image analysis
As reviewed in the last section, wetting the surface is expected to cause a variety of changes in color and luminance distributions. To investigate the actual optical changes, we measured and analyzed images of surfaces before and after wetting. 
Methods
Surface materials
Thirty-two materials were used for the analysis: clays, soils, cloths, wooden boards, and vegetables. We first took a photograph of each dry material. Then we dripped water onto the surface so that a thin water layer covered it and took a photograph of the wet surface. Thus, the spatial position in each wet and dry photograph was similar, although the water dripped onto the surface significantly changed its fine structure. Figure 2 shows several image examples. 
Figure 2
 
Photographs of dry and wet materials used in the image analysis. The color and luminance of the surface images were measured and analyzed before and after wetting.
Figure 2
 
Photographs of dry and wet materials used in the image analysis. The color and luminance of the surface images were measured and analyzed before and after wetting.
Camera settings
Photographs of dry and wet materials were taken using a standard digital camera (Nikon D5100) with a pixel resolution of 4928 × 3264 in a photography box illuminated by two fluorescent lamps (Toshiba FL20S-D-EDL-D65, Toshiba, Tokyo, Japan). The camera was set at 90° above each material. The two lamps were set so that they could produce natural ambient light. 
To calibrate the color of the camera image, the spectral sensitivities of the digital camera were measured by using a monochrometer (MINI-CHROM DMC1-03, Edmund Optics, Barrington, NJ) and a spectrometer (TOPCON SR-3, TOPCON, Tokyo, Japan). Specifically, the standard white target was illuminated by each monochromatic light, and the reflected light was captured by the digital camera and measured by the spectrometer. Since the spectral radiance of each monochromatic light changed with wavelength, we compensated for the difference using the spectral radiance measured by the spectrometer. The spectral sensitivities were measured across the whole visible wavelength spectrum from 380 to 780 nm with an interval of 5 nm. The relative sensitivities across the camera RGB channels were corrected by using the white checker in a Macbeth Color Checker. On the basis of the spectral sensitivities of the camera and the CIE-1931 two-degree color matching function, we calculated a 3 × 3 transformation matrix that converts the linear RGB to the XYZ coordinate. 
Using the transformation matrix, we calculated the color statistics of wet and dry surfaces. First, the linear RGB images were extracted by de-mosaicing a raw image of each photo, and the center regions of 1536 × 1536 were cropped. Then, the linear RGB was converted to XYZ and to uvY color coordinates to calculate the saturation in an equal-color space. The uv′ saturation suv and hue Huv were defined as follows:   where Display FormulaImage not available and Display FormulaImage not available indicate the uv′ chromaticity coordinates of the pixel at position (i, j) . The uv′ chromaticity coordinates of the white point (un, vn) were (0.2032, 0.4963), which corresponds to (0.3465, 0.3760) in the xy chromaticity coordinates. The white point was measured using the white checker in the Macbeth Color Checker positioned under the current lighting condition.  
Results
Figures 3 and 4 show the results of the image analysis. Figure 3a shows the color changes between the wet and dry surfaces. The horizontal axis indicates the saturation of the dry surface, while the vertical axis indicates that of the wet surface. Different symbols represent different surfaces. The results show that saturation is generally higher for wet surfaces than for dry ones (Figure 3a, left). On the other hand, surface hue is affected little by wetting (Figure 3a, right). In Figure 4, the color distribution in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates is separately shown for dry and wet surfaces. The white circle indicates the white point, and the distance between each dot and the white point indicates how saturated the color of each dot is. The green line in each figure indicates the distance averaged across all materials. The mean saturation across all materials in the wet condition is 1.43 times larger than in the dry condition. 
Figure 3
 
Color and luminance changes between the wet and dry surfaces. (a) The mean uv′ saturation (left) and the mean hue (right) of each wet surface are plotted as a function of those of each dry surface. The uv′ saturation and the hue were calculated using Equations 1 and 2, respectively. The mean hue was calculated using circular statistics (Equation 14). Results show that wetting a surface increases the saturation but has little effect on the hue. (b) The luminance statistics of each wet surface is plotted as a function of that of each dry surface. Different panels indicate different statistics as in the legend. The green line shows the relationship that there are no changes in the statistics between dry and wet surfaces. Results show that the mean luminance of the wet surface is lower than that of the dry surface. In addition, the skew of the luminance distribution tends to increase for wet surfaces in comparison with dry ones.
Figure 3
 
Color and luminance changes between the wet and dry surfaces. (a) The mean uv′ saturation (left) and the mean hue (right) of each wet surface are plotted as a function of those of each dry surface. The uv′ saturation and the hue were calculated using Equations 1 and 2, respectively. The mean hue was calculated using circular statistics (Equation 14). Results show that wetting a surface increases the saturation but has little effect on the hue. (b) The luminance statistics of each wet surface is plotted as a function of that of each dry surface. Different panels indicate different statistics as in the legend. The green line shows the relationship that there are no changes in the statistics between dry and wet surfaces. Results show that the mean luminance of the wet surface is lower than that of the dry surface. In addition, the skew of the luminance distribution tends to increase for wet surfaces in comparison with dry ones.
Figure 4
 
The color distribution of wet or dry surfaces in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates is shown in each panel. Different dots in the figure indicate different pixels of individual surfaces. The chromaticity values of all 32 surfaces are plotted in each figure. The white circle indicates the white point defined by the white checker in the Macbeth Color Checker. For both dry and wet surfaces, the mean chromaticity across all surfaces was close to the white point. The mean saturation across all materials in the wet condition, shown as the green line in each panel, was 1.43 times larger than that in the dry condition.
Figure 4
 
The color distribution of wet or dry surfaces in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates is shown in each panel. Different dots in the figure indicate different pixels of individual surfaces. The chromaticity values of all 32 surfaces are plotted in each figure. The white circle indicates the white point defined by the white checker in the Macbeth Color Checker. For both dry and wet surfaces, the mean chromaticity across all surfaces was close to the white point. The mean saturation across all materials in the wet condition, shown as the green line in each panel, was 1.43 times larger than that in the dry condition.
Figure 3b shows the luminance change between the wet and dry surfaces. First, the mean luminance of the wet surface is lower than that of the dry surface, which is consistent with previous studies (Twomey et al., 1986; Lekner & Dorf, 1988). In addition, the skew of the luminance distribution tends to increase for wet surfaces in comparison with dry ones, suggesting an increase in surface specular reflection (Jensen et al., 1999; Motoyoshi et al., 2007; Sharan, Li, Motoyoshi, Nishida, & Adelson, 2008). Other moment statistics (i.e., the standard deviation [SD] and kurtosis) are not affected by wetting in a systematic way. These results are consistent with the theoretical analysis described in the previous section. 
While we used surface images measured under diffuse illumination in the main analysis, we conducted an additional analysis where we strictly controlled the angular relationship of a directional light source and the surface orientation (Figure 5). By using a half mirror, a dry or wet material (a brick) was illuminated by a projector from the same direction with a color-calibrated camera. The surface slant of the brick was 0° or 20°. The camera was expected to receive strong specular reflections at 0° but not at 20°. 
Figure 5
 
Effects of specular highlights on luminance distributions of a wet surface. By using a half mirror, a dry or wet material (a brick) was illuminated by a projector from the same direction that images were taken with the color-calibrated camera (see also Appendix A for the detailed camera setting.) (a) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was parallel to the illumination/camera direction (Slant = 0°). In this condition, the specular reflection from the wet surface directly returns to the camera because the illumination direction was the same as the camera direction. The luminance distribution of the wet surface was more positively skewed than that of the dry surface. (b) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was slanted 20°, where the specular reflection was almost completely excluded. In this case, surface wetting simply darkened most of the pixels, making the luminance histogram slightly skewed in the negative direction.
Figure 5
 
Effects of specular highlights on luminance distributions of a wet surface. By using a half mirror, a dry or wet material (a brick) was illuminated by a projector from the same direction that images were taken with the color-calibrated camera (see also Appendix A for the detailed camera setting.) (a) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was parallel to the illumination/camera direction (Slant = 0°). In this condition, the specular reflection from the wet surface directly returns to the camera because the illumination direction was the same as the camera direction. The luminance distribution of the wet surface was more positively skewed than that of the dry surface. (b) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was slanted 20°, where the specular reflection was almost completely excluded. In this case, surface wetting simply darkened most of the pixels, making the luminance histogram slightly skewed in the negative direction.
Figure 5a and b show scatter plots of luminance values for the wet and dry surfaces. The results show that when the surface slant was 0° (Figure 5a), surface wetting darkened some pixels and brightened others, making the luminance histogram heavily skewed in the positive direction. On the other hand, when we nearly eliminated specular highlights at 20° (Figure 5b), surface wetting simply darkened most of the pixels, making the luminance histogram slightly skewed in the negative direction. These results agree with the theory that wetting a surface increases the specular highlights and darkens the diffuse reflections. Interestingly, a wet image that includes few specular highlights (Figure 5, lower) does not appear wet despite its being physically wet. 
With the same apparatus, we further analyzed the physical mechanism of the optical effects of wetting a surface through the method developed by Nayar, Krishnan, Grossberg, and Raskar (2006), which separates the direct and global reflection components of a scene using high-spatial-frequency illumination (see Appendix A for details). 
Wetness enhancing transformation
The image analysis showed that a wet surface tends to have higher saturation, lower mean luminance, and a more positively skewed luminance distribution than a dry one. To ascertain whether the human visual system uses these changes in the image features as cues to wet surface perception, we examined how they affect a natural dry scene. As shown in Figure 6, the right image was produced from the left one by (a) enhancing the saturation of pixel colors and (b) tone-remapping of luminance signals with an accelerating nonlinear function, which makes the intensity histogram positively skewed and decreases the mean luminance. We refer to these image changes as WET. Given that the image in Figure 6 is properly reproduced, we expect the dry scene on the left to appear wet on the right to many observers. To investigate the perceptual mechanism of surface wetness, we carried out a series of psychophysical experiments using this WET operator. 
Figure 6
 
Demonstration of the WET (wetness enhancing transformation) operator. The right image was produced from the left one by i) enhancing the saturation of pixel colors, and ii) tone-remapping of luminance signals with an accelerating nonlinear function, which makes the intensity histogram positively skewed, and decreases the mean luminance. This WET operator tends to make a dry surface look wetter.
Figure 6
 
Demonstration of the WET (wetness enhancing transformation) operator. The right image was produced from the left one by i) enhancing the saturation of pixel colors, and ii) tone-remapping of luminance signals with an accelerating nonlinear function, which makes the intensity histogram positively skewed, and decreases the mean luminance. This WET operator tends to make a dry surface look wetter.
Psychophysical experiments
Experiment 1a
First, we applied the WET operator to a variety of natural textures and evaluated how it affects the apparent image wetness in psychophysical experiments. We show some examples in Figure 7a and the more examples in Appendix B
Figure 7
 
The WET operator tends to make the original image look wetter, while the effect size varies depending on the image. (a) Stimulus examples of the original and the transformed images. The figure below each stimulus shows the hue histogram of the stimulus. The effect of the WET operator was strong for the upper two examples but weak for the lower one. (b) Results of the wetness-rating experiment. The horizontal axis indicates the rating for the original image, and the vertical axis indicates the rating for the transformed image. Each symbol indicates individual images. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 7
 
The WET operator tends to make the original image look wetter, while the effect size varies depending on the image. (a) Stimulus examples of the original and the transformed images. The figure below each stimulus shows the hue histogram of the stimulus. The effect of the WET operator was strong for the upper two examples but weak for the lower one. (b) Results of the wetness-rating experiment. The horizontal axis indicates the rating for the original image, and the vertical axis indicates the rating for the transformed image. Each symbol indicates individual images. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Methods
Unless otherwise stated, the methods used in the subsequent experiments were the same as described in this section. 
Participants:
Thirteen paid volunteers participated. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The participants were naive to the purpose and methods of the experiment. The experiments were approved by the Ethical Committees at NTT Communication Science Laboratories and were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Apparatus:
The experimental stimuli were displayed using Matlab R2013b in conjunction with the Psychophysics Toolbox 3 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). They were displayed on a calibrated 30-in. LCD monitor (EIZO, ColorEdge CG303W) controlled with an NVIDIA video card (Quadro 600) with a pixel resolution of 2560 × 1600. The intensity of each phosphor was linearly corrected and could be varied with 10-bit resolution. The maximum luminance of the monitor was 150.6 cd/m2. The xy coordinates of the white point were (0.3331, 0.3615). A participant viewed the stimuli in a dark room at a viewing distance of 57 cm. 
Stimuli:
Eighty-eight images were used in Experiment 1a. These images were taken from the Textures category of the McGill Calibrated Colour Image Database (Olmos & Kingdom, 2004; Figure 7a). The stimulus examples are also shown in Appendix B. The WET operator changed the luminance of the original image Yoriginal (which had been normalized in the range from 0 to 1) into Ywet as follows:  where (i, j) indicates the pixel position. The coordinates of white (un, vn) for calculating the saturation suv in Equation 1 were (0.1998, 0.4874). To increase the luminance skew, we set gamma = 3. To enhance the saturation of the texture, we added 0.26 to the suv of each pixel. In deciding these parameters of the WET transformation, we preliminary explored a range of parameters and chose ones that did not disturb the apparent naturalness of all the images used in Experiment 1a.  
Procedures:
In the experiments, images selected from the stimulus set were presented one by one. The image sizes were 18.6° × 14.2° in Experiments 1a and 1c (Figures 7 and 9) and 12° × 12° in Experiment 1b (Figure 8). Observers rated how wet the surface image appeared on a 5-point scale. At the beginning of the experiment, we showed the observers sample pictures of wet postrain outdoor scenes together with the pictures of dry scenes. Most of the wet scenes included direct evidence of water such as puddles or water drops. Using these “strongly wet” pictures as the reference for the score “5” and dry pictures as that for the score “1,” we instructed the observers to evaluate the magnitude of wetness in the presented stimulus image. Each stimulus condition was tested 10 times in total for each observer. 
Figure 8
 
Contributions of color saturation and luminance distribution skew to wet perception. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 1b, where we continuously and independently manipulated the color saturation and the luminance histogram skew of a natural surface using histogram matching. (b) Results of Experiment 1b. The horizontal axis indicates the mean saturation defined by Equation 1, and the vertical axis indicates the skew of the luminance histogram. The size of the red circles in each panel indicates the mean rating averaged across observers. The width of the blue contour in each panel indicates + 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that both saturation and luminance skew contribute to enhancing the wetness impression by the WET operator. (c) Separate plots for the effects of the luminance skew and the color saturation. In the left panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across color saturation conditions are plotted as a function of the luminance skew. In the right panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across the luminance skew conditions are plotted as a function of the color saturation. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 8
 
Contributions of color saturation and luminance distribution skew to wet perception. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 1b, where we continuously and independently manipulated the color saturation and the luminance histogram skew of a natural surface using histogram matching. (b) Results of Experiment 1b. The horizontal axis indicates the mean saturation defined by Equation 1, and the vertical axis indicates the skew of the luminance histogram. The size of the red circles in each panel indicates the mean rating averaged across observers. The width of the blue contour in each panel indicates + 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that both saturation and luminance skew contribute to enhancing the wetness impression by the WET operator. (c) Separate plots for the effects of the luminance skew and the color saturation. In the left panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across color saturation conditions are plotted as a function of the luminance skew. In the right panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across the luminance skew conditions are plotted as a function of the color saturation. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 9
 
The effect of chroma, another definition of the color saturation. For the two images used in Experiment 1a (Surface 1 and 2), we continuously modulated the uv′ saturation and measured the perceived wetness. The results are plotted as a function of the mean chroma of each stimulus. The vertical orange line indicates the mean chroma of the original image. The horizontal green line indicates the mean rating in Experiment 1a for the original image. Different symbols in each panel indicate different skew conditions. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that the wetting impression increases with color saturation even when chroma is used as the definition of saturation.
Figure 9
 
The effect of chroma, another definition of the color saturation. For the two images used in Experiment 1a (Surface 1 and 2), we continuously modulated the uv′ saturation and measured the perceived wetness. The results are plotted as a function of the mean chroma of each stimulus. The vertical orange line indicates the mean chroma of the original image. The horizontal green line indicates the mean rating in Experiment 1a for the original image. Different symbols in each panel indicate different skew conditions. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that the wetting impression increases with color saturation even when chroma is used as the definition of saturation.
Results
Figure 7b shows the results of Experiment 1a. The horizontal axis indicates the wetness rating for the original surface image, and the vertical axis indicates the rating for the transformed image. A one-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the WET operator (original or transformed) as the within-subjects variable showed a significant main effect, F(1, 12) = 71.384, p < 0.0001). The results show that the perceived wetness was on average much stronger for the transformed images than for the original ones (Figure 7b). In this experiment, the wet rating score of the transformed images was not very high. One reason may be that we used “mild” WET parameters to warrant the naturalness of all the transformed images. Another reason may be that the WET operator does not create the water drops or puddles included in the reference images for the highest score. 
Experiment 1b
The luminance transformation of WET is exponential; therefore, the operator not only increases the skew of the luminance distribution but also decreases the mean. To clarify what factor was critical for the wetness perception, we used a histogram-matching method to change the luminance skew of an image by while keeping the luminance mean constant (Figure 8a). In addition, to separately evaluate the contribution of chromatic/achromatic factors of the WET operator, we continuously and independently manipulated the color saturation and the luminance skew of a natural texture. 
Methods
Twelve paid volunteers participated. The luminance skew of an image was changed while the luminance mean was kept constant by using a histogram-matching method (Heeger & Bergen, 1995). The reference distribution for the matching was a beta distribution, and the skew of the distribution was set from −1.5 to 1.5 in 0.5 steps. The mean luminance and the standard deviation of the surface were set to 0.3 and 0.1, respectively. The saturation distribution of each surface was replaced with a Gaussian distribution using the histogram-matching method. The mean saturation of the Gaussian distribution ranged from 0.026 to 0.36 in 0.026 steps. The standard deviation was set to 0.013. In addition, a monochromatic surface (suv = 0) was also used. In the experiment, a surface image taken with a standard digital camera (Nikon D5100) was used (Figure 8). 
Results
Figure 8b shows the results. The horizontal axis indicates the mean saturation, and the vertical axis indicates the skew of the luminance histogram. The size of the red circle in each panel indicates the mean rating averaged across observers. Figure 8c shows the main effect of the luminance skew and that of the mean saturation separately. We conducted a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with the skew and saturation conditions as factors. The main effects of the skew and saturation conditions and the interaction were statistically significant, F(6, 66) = 28.504, p < 0.0001; F(14, 154) = 35.741, p < 0.0001; and F(84, 924) = 2.3671, p < 0.0001, respectively). The results show that the skew modulation was effective for the wetness perception even without the mean luminance change. Furthermore, over the whole range we tested, an increase in the luminance skew, or in the color saturation, monotonically increased the wetness impression (Figure 8c). It is noteworthy that the wetness rating approached the highest score (5) when the luminance skew was the most positive and the color saturation was the highest. 
Experiment 1c
There is more than one definition of color saturation. The uv′ saturation, the one we used, is independent of luminance, whereas another definition, chroma C*uv, is luminance-dependent as follows:     where Display FormulaImage not available and Display FormulaImage not available are the chromaticity in the CIE L*u*v* color space and Display FormulaImage not available indicates lightness in the space. Display FormulaImage not available and Display FormulaImage not available are the luminance values of each pixel and the white point, respectively. Chroma C*uv is defined as the distance of each pixel chromaticity in the u*v* plane from the white point (0, 0). Since the definitions of u* and v* are luminance-dependent, chroma C*uv is also luminance-dependent. By combining Equations 47 with Equation 1, chroma C*uv can be described as follows:    
The WET operator used in Experiment 1a decreased the mean luminance and thus worked to decrease chroma C*uv. Therefore, one might argue that the saturation enhancement was needed only to compensate for the decrease in chroma due to the luminance modulation. To confirm whether an increase in chroma is necessary to obtain a wetting effect, we continuously changed the saturation of the original image more widely than we did in Experiment 1b. 
Methods
Eight paid volunteers participated. The uv′ saturation of two surface images in McGill textures was continuously modulated by a histogram-matching method. The methods of the transformation were the same as in Experiment 1b. The mean uv′ saturation of each image ranged from 0.065 to 0.52 in 0.065 steps. The standard deviation was set to 0.013. In addition, the skew of each luminance distribution was set to −0.8 and 0.8. The mean luminance of the surface image was set to 0.33. The standard deviation of the two surfaces was set to 0.09 and 0.07, respectively. 
Results
Figure 9 shows the results. The horizontal axis indicates the mean chroma C*uv of each stimulus, and the vertical axis indicates the wetness rating averaged across observers. The vertical orange line in each panel shows the chroma C*uv of the original image. The results show that when the mean chroma of the transformed image was around the original chroma, a strong wetting impression could not be obtained even if the luminance skew was modulated. In addition, the wetting impression increased even when the mean chroma of the transformed image was higher than that of the original image. These findings suggest that an increase in chroma is necessary to obtain a strong wetting effect. 
Why is the WET operator more effective on some images than on others?
We have emphasized an explanation of the WET operator based on ecological optics. That is, human vision implicitly knows characteristic optical changes produced by surface wetting. Since the WET operator simulates these changes, it makes surface images look wetter to human observers. However, this is only half of what we can learn about wet processing from the present data. In Experiment 1a, we found a significant variation in the effect size of the WET operator (Figure 7b). The image transformation greatly increased the wetness rating for some images but not for others. What image factor determines the effect size of the WET operator? Using a multiple regression analysis, we evaluated the contribution of a wide range of image statistics of the original (untransformed) images. 
Methods
We performed a multiple regression analysis for the ratings. To avoid overfitting, we used the stepwise method, along with the Lasso regression, a multiple linear regression with a shrinkage and selection constraint, to select critical variables (Tibshirani, 1996). In both cases, we regressed a variety of image statistics of the original image to explain the effect sizes of the transformation (i.e., the differences between wetness ratings of the transformed and original images). As the predictor variables, we used the mean, standard deviation, skew, kurtosis, and global entropy of luminance, saturation, or hue in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates. 
For luminance and saturation, the statistics are defined as follows:      where I(p) is the pixel value at the pixel position p and Display FormulaImage not available is the number of pixels. The g is the discrete pixel level, which ranges from 0 to (G-1), and P(g) is the probability density of the level g on the pixel histogram.  
Since hue is a circular variable, we used circular statistics (Berens, 2009) as follows:     where α(p) is the pixel hue at the pixel position p and Display FormulaImage not available is the number of pixels. The hue entropy is calculated using Equation 13 because it is not affected by circular variables.  
Since the entropy in Equation 13 ignores the spatial component of an image, we also used the spatial entropy (Brink, 1995; Razlighi & Kehtarnavaz, 2009) of luminance, saturation, or hue image as the predictor variable. For the spatial entropy, the joint probability distribution function Pgg′(k,l) between samples of Display FormulaImage not available and Display FormulaImage not available was used to compute  which corresponds to the probability that a pixel value g occurs at distance (k, l) from another pixel value g′. In the present analysis, the distance (k, l) is within the 3 × 3 neighborhood of the pixel position (i, j). For each (k, l), an entropy value H(k,l) is defined as follows:    
Since H(k,l) ranges from H(0,0) to 2H(0, 0), which corresponds to the entropy defined in Equation 13 and twice the entropy, respectively, relative entropy Display FormulaImage not available is defined as follows.    
By simply summing the entropy values for all (k, l), the spatial entropy value Display FormulaImage not available was obtained. To see the spatial entropy on multiple scales, we computed Display FormulaImage not available for images scaled down to 1/1, 1/2, 1/4, 1/8, 1/16, and 1/32. For all the entropy computations, the number of histogram bins (i.e., the resolution of I) was set to 512.  
Thus, 33 of the predictor variables in total were used in the present regression. Before the regression analyses, each predictor variable was normalized to z scores. 
Results
Figure 10 shows the results of the single regression for each feature, stepwise multiple regression, and Lasso regression. The results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis show significant contributions of luminance histogram skew, luminance spatial entropy, hue (global) entropy, and hue spatial entropy. The results of the Lasso regression (Tibshirani, 1996) support strong positive contributions of hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy (Figure 10). 
Figure 10
 
Regression analysis as to which image features contribute to the effect size of WET. (a) Results of the single regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant correlations with the effect size (p < 0.05). (a) Results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant weights (p < 0.05). The results indicate significant contributions of luminance histogram skew, luminance spatial entropy, hue (global) entropy, and hue spatial entropy. (b) Results of the lasso regression analysis. The results support strong positive contributions of hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy.
Figure 10
 
Regression analysis as to which image features contribute to the effect size of WET. (a) Results of the single regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant correlations with the effect size (p < 0.05). (a) Results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant weights (p < 0.05). The results indicate significant contributions of luminance histogram skew, luminance spatial entropy, hue (global) entropy, and hue spatial entropy. (b) Results of the lasso regression analysis. The results support strong positive contributions of hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy.
Entropy tells us how much information there is in an image. Specifically, hue entropy is related to the variation of the hue distribution. A large value indicates that the hue histogram has a nearly uniform distribution (Figure 7a, upper), while a small value indicates that it has a biased distribution (Figure 7a, lower). On the other hand, luminance spatial entropy is related to the spatial variation of the luminance pattern. The positive contributions of the hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy to the effect size of the WET operator suggests that it is more effective for images with a larger number of hues and a larger magnitude of spatial variation. It should be noted that the WET operator itself does not change the hue and luminance entropies. The results suggest that the role of the entropy is to modulate the effects of color saturation and luminance skew on the wet perception, rather than to directly alter the wet perception. 
Experiment 2
Motivated by the results in Figure 10, we experimentally explored the genuine contribution of the hue entropy to the wetness perception. We synthesized artificial Mondrian-like three-dimensional (3-D) scenes using a physically based renderer (Jakob, 2010) and investigated their appearance. 
Methods
Stimuli:
The original 3-D Mondrian images were made by using the physically based renderer Mitsuba (Jakob, 2010). In a 3-D space, a camera was set 1.5 m from a plain gray surface 3 × 3 m in size, and 750 small cubes were randomly placed in front of it. The width, height, and depth of each cube were randomly selected in a range from 0.01 to 0.1 m. The bidirectional reflectance distribution function (BRDF) of each cube was Lambertian, and its reflectance was defined in the spectral domain. The mean reflectance of each cube was randomly modulated. However, to control the hue entropy of the output image, we used three entropy conditions—small, intermediate, and large entropy—for the peak in the reflectance spectra of the cube (Figure 11a, upper). For all the conditions, the reflectance spectra R(λ) of each cube were defined as follows:  where the λ is the wavelength of the light, which ranged from 380 to 780 nm, λp is a peak in reflectance spectra, and σ is the standard deviation, which was set to 20 nm. The a2 is a contrast factor, and it was set to 0.3. The a1 is pedestal reflectance, and it was randomly selected from 0.3 to 0.5. For the small entropy condition, the peak in reflectance spectra λp of each cube was always 700 nm. For the middle entropy condition, a peak in reflectance spectra λp of each cube was randomly selected from 546 or 700 nm. For the large entropy condition, the peak was randomly selected from 400 to 700 nm. The scenes were rendered by using the photon-mapping algorithm (Jensen, 2001). We used as the environment emitter an environment map downloaded from Bernhard Vogl's website (“At the Window (Wells, UK)”, http://dativ.at/lightprobes/), with the color converted to grayscale. The hue global entropy of the output image (Equation 13) in the small, intermediate, or large entropy condition was 2.8, 4.8, or 6.6, respectively. The transformed images were made (Figure 11a, lower) by applying the WET operator to the original images. Each luminance signal of the original image, which had been normalized in the range from 0 to 1, was transformed by using Equation 3. Each saturation signal of the image was defined as in Equation 1 and multiplied by a factor of three.  
Figure 11
 
Effects of the WET operator depend on the hue entropy. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 2. We used a physically based renderer to make the original images. By modulating the peak in reflectance spectra of each object, we controlled the hue entropy of the image. The hue entropy of each image under the small, middle, or large hue entropy conditions was 2.8, 4.8, or 6.6, respectively. To make the transformed image, we applied the WET operator to the original image. (b) Results of the wetness judgment (left) and darkness judgment (right). Ratings of the pairwise comparison were converted into a one-dimensional perceptual scale. The results indicate that the wetness perception of a scene image increases with the hue entropy of the image without changes in the luminance spatial entropy or in other factors.
Figure 11
 
Effects of the WET operator depend on the hue entropy. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 2. We used a physically based renderer to make the original images. By modulating the peak in reflectance spectra of each object, we controlled the hue entropy of the image. The hue entropy of each image under the small, middle, or large hue entropy conditions was 2.8, 4.8, or 6.6, respectively. To make the transformed image, we applied the WET operator to the original image. (b) Results of the wetness judgment (left) and darkness judgment (right). Ratings of the pairwise comparison were converted into a one-dimensional perceptual scale. The results indicate that the wetness perception of a scene image increases with the hue entropy of the image without changes in the luminance spatial entropy or in other factors.
Procedure:
The observers' task (eight paid volunteers) was to judge wetness (or darkness). In each trial, the observers viewed a pair of the scene images in Figure 11a. The size of each image was 12.6° × 12.6°. The observers were asked to select the surface images that looked wetter (or darker) using a 5-point scale: A rating of 0 indicated no difference between the pair in perceived wetness (or darkness); a rating of ±1 indicated that the left/right image is slightly wetter (or darker) than the right/left one; a rating of ±2 indicated that the left/right image is much wetter (or darker) than the right/left one. Each stimulus condition was tested 20 times for each observer. 
Analysis of the results:
Following Scheffé (1952), we calculated the perceived scale values Q(m) for each stimulus condition using the following formulas:   where M indicates the mean rating for the stimulus pair (m, n) or (n, m) and P(m,n) is the mean rating for m over n, averaged over the two orders. Q(m) is the perceived scale value of stimulus condition m. We calculated the perceived scale values for each observer in the wetness rating and the darkness rating (Figure 11b).  
Results
The ratings for the stimulus pairs within each observer were analyzed by using Equations 22 and 23. The calculated value of the perceived wetness or darkness is shown in the Figure 11b. Different symbols indicate different stimulus conditions. We conducted a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the wetness rating, with entropy condition (small, intermediate, or large) and WET condition (original or transformed) as factors. The main effects of the entropy and WET conditions and the interaction were statistically significant, F(2, 14) = 11.942, p < 0.0001, F(1, 7) = 189.766, p < 0.0001, and F(2, 14) = 8.244, p = 0.004, respectively. The results show that the wetness perception of a scene image increased with the hue entropy of the image without changes in the luminance spatial entropy or in other factors. Specifically, the perceived wetness of the transformed image in the large entropy condition (Figure 11, open blue circle) was statistically higher than in the small and intermediate entropy conditions (Figure 11, open red circle and open green circle), t(7) = 4.988, p = 0.005 for small entropy, and t(7) = 3.338, p = 0.038 for intermediate entropy (Bonferroni-corrected paired t test), but there was no significant difference between the wetness values of the transformed image in the small and intermediate entropy conditions, t(7) = 0.2494, p = 0.748 (Bonferroni-corrected paired t test). 
Furthermore, the transformed images were generally perceived as wetter than the original ones. The results can be understood by considering the spatial complexity of the luminance distribution. As in the results of Experiment 1a, the spatial entropy of the luminance distribution can contribute to the perceived wetness (Figure 10). The stimulus used in Experiment 2 was highly cluttered and had higher spatial entropy. 
In addition, using the same stimulus set, we found that the hue entropy did not affect the darkness rating (Figure 11b, right). The dissociation of wetness and darkness ratings excludes the possibility that the observers simply judged the darkness of a surface to estimate its wetness. 
While we used 3-D Mondrian patterns in the main experiment, we also tested two-dimensional (2-D) ones (Figure 12) in a preliminary experiment. Some observers showed results similar to those obtained with 3-D patterns, but others had trouble judging wetness for the 2-D patterns. Although wetness is a surface/object property, the 2-D patterns did not look like collections of surfaces/objects to the latter group of observers. We therefore used 3-D patterns, for which all observers could easily make judgments on the wetness dimension. 
Figure 12
 
Two-dimensional (2-D) Mondrian patterns used in a preliminary test of Experiment 2. Some observers showed results similar to those obtained with 3-D patterns, but others had trouble judging wetness for the 2-D patterns.
Figure 12
 
Two-dimensional (2-D) Mondrian patterns used in a preliminary test of Experiment 2. Some observers showed results similar to those obtained with 3-D patterns, but others had trouble judging wetness for the 2-D patterns.
Experiment 3
Wetting a surface does mainly three things: It increases color saturation, increases luminance skew, and decreases mean luminance (Figure 3). We have shown that the first two factors effectively change apparent wetness without any changes in the mean luminance (see Experiments 1b), but how much impact the mean luminance reduction has on apparent wetness remains unclear. The final experiment investigated this issue using the 3-D Mondrian stimuli used in Experiment 2. 
Methods
The original image was a high-hue entropy scene (Figure 13a, upper left). In one condition, the mean luminance was decreased by multiplying the pixel intensities by a constant factor of 0.2 while the luminance skew was held constant (Figure 13a, upper center). In the other condition, the luminance gamma was increased to simultaneously reduce the mean luminance and increase the luminance skew (upper right), as in the original WET operator. The mean luminance was equated between the two conditions. The color saturation was equated either to the original image or to a saturation-doubled image. The observers' task (eight paid volunteers) was to judge wetness (or darkness). The other methods were the same as in Experiment 2. 
Figure 13
 
(a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 3. (b) Results of Experiment 3. The results indicate that decreasing the mean luminance (green) is not sufficient for significantly increasing the wetting impression.
Figure 13
 
(a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 3. (b) Results of Experiment 3. The results indicate that decreasing the mean luminance (green) is not sufficient for significantly increasing the wetting impression.
Results
The perceived wetness or darkness, calculated in the same way as in Experiment 2, is shown in the Figure 13b. Different symbols indicate different stimulus conditions. We conducted a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the wetness and darkness ratings, with luminance condition (original, mean decreased, or gamma modulated) and saturation condition (original or highly saturated) as factors. For the wetness rating, the main effects of the luminance condition was statistically significant, F(2, 14) = 26.440, p < 0.0001, while the main effect of the saturation condition and the interaction were not, F(1, 7) = 2.665, p = 0.147, and F(2, 14) = 1.519, p = 0.253, respectively. For the darkness rating, the main effects of the luminance and the saturation conditions were statistically significant, F(2, 14) = 73.534, p < 0.0001 and F(1, 7) = 15.078, p = 0.006, respectively), while the interaction was not, F(2, 14) = 0.382, p = 0.689. The results show that decreasing the mean luminance (Figure 13, green) was not enough to significantly increase the wetting impression. Specifically, regardless of the magnitude of color saturation, the apparent darkness did, t(7) = 6.938, p = 0.0006 (Bonferroni-corrected paired t test), but the apparent wetness did not, t(7) = 2.130, p = 0.21 (Bonferroni-corrected paired t test) show significant differences between the original image (Figure 13, red) and the mean-decreased image (Figure 13, green). In contrast, the original WET operator (Figure 13, blue) produced a strong wetting effect, t(7) = 5.341, p = 0.003 (Bonferroni-corrected paired t test). 
General discussion
The present study investigated human perception of surface wetness by analyzing the physical processes related to surface wetting and the image features sensitive to the visual processing of wetness. The image analysis using photographs of dry and wet surfaces showed that the latter tended to have higher saturation and more positively skewed luminance distribution than the former. Psychophysical experiments showed that the WET operator that modulated the color and luminance statistics as in a wet surface tended to make a dry surface look wetter in general. These findings suggest that to some degree human wetness perception relies on the ecological optics underlying surface wetting. In addition, these experiments suggest that the effect size of the WET operator depends on the hue entropy of the image, or a similar measure of the spread of color distribution. 
The effect of hue entropy cannot be explained by ecological optics, since wet-related changes in color saturation and luminance histograms take place regardless of the number of colors in the scene. Instead, hue entropy may reflect the perceptual computation the brain adopts to evaluate surface wetness. Even when wet image features are recognized in the image, the observers cannot tell whether they are indeed caused by surface wetness (Figure 14a). This is because similar image features could be caused by other events—for instance, when the visual scene happens to include objects with highly color-saturated glossy surfaces. In addition, as shown by Figures 3 and 4, wet-related changes in color and luminance statistics are small relative to their variances. Since there is a significant overlap in the distribution of color saturation between dry and wet samples, it seems it was hard for observers to tell whether one color sample is taken from a dry image or a wet image. The key to resolving these ambiguities is to increase the number of samples. When wet-related image features are shared by many different parts in the scene, the image features are likely to be produced by a global common factor such as wetting. In other words, the more independent colors the scene contains, the more reliably the visual system can judge the scene wetness. 
Figure 14
 
Hue entropy can reduce the ambiguity of physical events. (a) Schematic explanations of the relationship between physical events and image features. Besides surface wetness, other factors, such as the existence of highly color-saturated glossy surfaces, can produce similar image features. This is why the optical image features of luminance and saturation are not sufficient for estimating that the surface is wet. (b) Explanation of a framework that can explain the effects of hue entropy. Each colored panel indicates the probabilities of the occurrence of a wet or dry scene. As in Figure 4, we here assume that the probabilities of the occurrence for wet colors spread 1.43 times more widely than that for dry colors. The upper and lower panels indicate the small and large hue entropy conditions, respectively. For the large hue entropy, the posterior probability for the wet scene can be larger than that for the dry scene, while it tends to be smaller for the small hue entropy.
Figure 14
 
Hue entropy can reduce the ambiguity of physical events. (a) Schematic explanations of the relationship between physical events and image features. Besides surface wetness, other factors, such as the existence of highly color-saturated glossy surfaces, can produce similar image features. This is why the optical image features of luminance and saturation are not sufficient for estimating that the surface is wet. (b) Explanation of a framework that can explain the effects of hue entropy. Each colored panel indicates the probabilities of the occurrence of a wet or dry scene. As in Figure 4, we here assume that the probabilities of the occurrence for wet colors spread 1.43 times more widely than that for dry colors. The upper and lower panels indicate the small and large hue entropy conditions, respectively. For the large hue entropy, the posterior probability for the wet scene can be larger than that for the dry scene, while it tends to be smaller for the small hue entropy.
This explanation may have a relationship with a Bayesian inference model (Figure 14b; e.g., Knill & Richards, 1996; Weiss, Simoncelli, & Adelson, 2002). The problem the observer has to solve is to classify a scene into dry or wet on the basis of the difference in the color saturation distribution. The saturation is generally higher for wet scenes than for dry ones. The probability distributions of the saturation for the dry and wet scenes, P(S|Dry) and P(S|Wet), are something like those shown in Figure 4. With regard to the prior probability, we assume that the scene is more likely to be dry than wet: P(Dry) > P(Wet). The scene S is judge as wet when P(Dry|S) < P(Wet|S):   where N is the number of independent colors present in the scene, which usually increases with the hue entropy.  
According to this model, when the number of independent colors is small (the hue entropy of the image or the amount of color information is low) and the sampled colors are saturated, the scene is judged to be dry because of the difference in the prior probability. On the other hand, when the number of independent colors is large (hue entropy is high) and sampled colors are all saturated, the scene is judged to be wet because of the increased contribution of the likelihood. For instance, let us consider a simple situation where (a) there are only two color saturation states, low and high; (b) the likelihood of high saturation is higher in the wet condition than in the dry condition, P(Shigh|Dry) = 0.4, P(Slow|Dry) = 0.6, P(Shigh|Wet) = 0.5, P(Slow|Wet) = 0.5; and (c) the prior probability is higher for the dry condition than for the wet condition, P(Dry) = 0.7 and P(Wet) = 0.3. When there is only one color sample and it has high saturation, the model predicts dry, since P(Dry|Shigh) = 0.28 > P(Wet|Shigh) = 0.15. On the other hand, when there are five samples and all of them have high saturation, the model predicts wet, since P(Dry|Shigh) = 0.0072 < P(Wet|Shigh) = 0.0094. 
We also consider the model behavior under a more realistic assumption: (a) the probability distribution of color is widely spread and approximated by a Gaussian function centered around the white point; and (b) the standard deviation of the Gaussian distribution is ×1.43 larger (the average saturation is ×1.43 higher) for the wet condition than for the dry one. For a given dry/wet image, N independent colors are randomly sampled from the dry/wet distribution. By repeating this 1,024 times, we obtained the frequency distribution histogram of the combination of ΠP(Sx|Dry) and ΠP(Sx|Wet) as shown in Figure 15. The blue histogram was obtained when N color samples were randomly taken from the wet color distribution; the red one when they were randomly taken from the dry color distribution. Both histograms are aligned on a single line for each N. The dotted line is the boundary above which the likelihood is higher for wet than for dry. When the number of samples is one (N = 1), the blue and red histograms significantly overlap with each other. It is hard for the observer to tell whether the sample is from the wet or dry distribution only from the likelihood terms (and the prior produces a decision bias for dry). As the number of samples is increased, however, the blue and red distributions are gradually separated from each other, making it easier to tell whether the color samples are from the wet or dry distribution (unless a strong prior bias in favor of dry overrides the difference in the likelihood terms). The model nicely explains how an increase in hue entropy, or in the number of different color samples, elevates the reliability of color saturation as a cue to judge surface wetness. 
Figure 15
 
The results of a simulation of the model in Figure 14 under an assumption that the probability distribution of color is approximated by a Gaussian function, with the spread's being ×1.43 wider for the wet condition than for the dry condition. A diagonal view (a) and a top view (b) of the frequency distribution histogram of the combination of ΠP(Sx|Dry) and ΠP(Sx|Wet). The blue histogram was obtained when N color samples were randomly taken from the wet color distribution; the red histogram was obtained when they were randomly taken from the dry color distribution. The dotted line is the boundary above which the likelihood is higher for wet than for dry. When the number of samples is one (N = 1), blue and red histograms significantly overlap each other. As the number of samples is increased, however, the blue and red distributions are gradually separated from each other, making it easier to tell whether the color samples are from the wet or dry distribution.
Figure 15
 
The results of a simulation of the model in Figure 14 under an assumption that the probability distribution of color is approximated by a Gaussian function, with the spread's being ×1.43 wider for the wet condition than for the dry condition. A diagonal view (a) and a top view (b) of the frequency distribution histogram of the combination of ΠP(Sx|Dry) and ΠP(Sx|Wet). The blue histogram was obtained when N color samples were randomly taken from the wet color distribution; the red histogram was obtained when they were randomly taken from the dry color distribution. The dotted line is the boundary above which the likelihood is higher for wet than for dry. When the number of samples is one (N = 1), blue and red histograms significantly overlap each other. As the number of samples is increased, however, the blue and red distributions are gradually separated from each other, making it easier to tell whether the color samples are from the wet or dry distribution.
However, it should be noted that we propose this model only for the purpose of giving insight into the effect of hue entropy on wetness perception. We do not expect the model, at least the current version, to be able to precisely predict human wetness perception. One limitation is that it assumes an ideal observer who perfectly knows the natural color statistics. In addition, the model does not take into account the contribution of many other cues to wetness perception. 
Although the present study focused on the role of color and luminance statistics of the image in human wetness perception, we do not claim that the visual system can evaluate the wetness of an image only by using those image statistics, nor that those statistics are powerful enough to predict human wet/dry judgments regardless of other stimulus conditions. Our claim is that, all else being equal, changing the statistics tends to change the wetness in a predictable way. The idea is easy to understand when we consider the example of classical cues. For instance, height in visual field does not, by itself, predict apparent depth. However, in a given picture, increasing an object's in height in visual field tends to increase its apparent distance. The angular subtense of an object on the page does not, by itself, predict its apparent 3-D size. However, increasing on object's angular subtense tends to increase its apparent 3-D size. Analogously, the color saturation of an image may not, by itself, predict the apparent magnitude of wetness, but increasing it tends to make a dry surface look wet. Therefore, color saturation is a cue to perceive wetness, as an object's height is a cue to perceive depth. 
Wetting real objects often produces a characteristic shape change. For instance, when a hair-like object is wet, the absorbed water makes the object heavier and alters its chemical properties (Rungjiratananon et al., 2012). The resulting shape change should be an effective cue for the wetness perception. Wet scenes often contain direct visual evidence of water, such as drops and puddles. For a fuller understanding of human wetness perception, the contribution of the nonoptical factors should be taken into account. 
The present case study for wetness perception demonstrates how useful color image statistics could be for estimating physical surface conditions or other events that alter the spectral characteristics of light reflectance. In this regard, the present finding is related to the effects of color on intrinsic image decomposition. It is known that human observers tend to interpret luminance spatial changes accompanied by color changes as material changes, while they tend to interpret those orthogonal to color changes as illumination changes (Kingdom, 2003). This suggests that the visual system “knows” that color changes are usually caused by material changes, but that luminance changes can be caused be either material or illumination changes. Human observers also use color information to segregate highlights from body color, apparently exploiting the differences in the spectral properties of diffuse and specular reflections (Nishida, Motoyoshi, & Maruya, 2011). Together with these previous findings, the present finding suggests that the brain makes use of color information not only for perceiving color but also for decoding the optics of the external world embedded in the retinal image. 
The WET operator is a practically useful image-processing tool. It can quickly change a dry scene into a wet one by a simple image manipulation. One might then wonder if an inverse function of the WET operator can make a wet surface look dryer. The results of Experiment 1b suggest that changing the parameters of the WET operator in the opposite direction enhances apparent dryness. Furthermore, as shown in Figure 16, we observed that the dryness enhancing transformation (DET) was able to make some real wet scenes look dryer. However, the DET would be more limited than the WET because it cannot undo/remove nonoptical changes produced by actual wetting, such as shape changes and water drops. 
Figure 16
 
Examples of the DET operator. The DET can tend to make the original images look dryer.
Figure 16
 
Examples of the DET operator. The DET can tend to make the original images look dryer.
To conclude, the human visual system tends to perceive surface wetness when the color saturations are high, the luminance histogram is positively skewed, and the hue entropy is high. The first two conditions agree with actual image changes produced by surface wetting (ecological optics), while the last condition can be explained by a Bayesian-like inference that takes into account the probability of dry or wet surfaces in the environment. The present findings not only provide a novel view of human wetness perception, but also reveal how elegantly human vision uses low-level color statistics to estimate complex optical properties in the real world. A scientific understanding of the strategies the human visual system takes to estimate surface wetness (see also Sawayama & Kimura, 2015) will lead to the development of techniques to synthesize, edit, and recognize wet scenes that are much simpler than physics-based ones (Jensen et al., 1999; Lenaerts, Adams, & Dutré, 2008; Chen, Thalmann, & Allen, 2012; Rungjiratananon et al., 2012). 
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas from Japan Society of Promotion of Science to SN (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP15H05915) and by a grant from the National Science Foundation to EHA. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Masataka Sawayama. 
Address: NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation, Kanagawa, Japan. 
References
Arce-Lopera, C., Masuda, T., Kimura, A., Wada, Y., & Okajima, K. (2012). Luminance distribution modifies the perceived freshness of strawberries. i-Perception, 3 (5), 338–355.
Arend, L., & Reeves, A. (1986). Simultaneous color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 3 (10), 1743–1751.
Berens, P. (2009). CircStat: A MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. Journal of Statistical Software, 31 (10), 1–21.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10 (4), 433–436.
Brainard, D. H., & Freeman, W. T. (1997). Bayesian color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 14 (7), 1393–1411.
Brink, A. (1995). Minimum spatial entropy threshold selection. IEEE Proceedings - Vision, Image and Signal Processing, 142 (3), 128–132.
Chen, Y., Thalmann, N. M., & Allen, B. F. (2012). Physical simulation of wet clothing for virtual humans. The Visual Computer, 28 (6–8), 765–774.
Heeger, D., & Bergen, J. (1995). Pyramid-based texture analysis/synthesis. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques (pp. 229–238). New York: ACM.
Jakob, W. (2010). Mitsuba renderer. Retrieved from http://www.mitsuba-renderer.org
Jensen, H. W. (2001). Realistic image synthesis using photon mapping. Natick, MA: A. K. Peters.
Jensen, H. W., Legakis, J., & Dorsey, J. (1999). Rendering of wet materials. In Rendering Techniques '99 (pp. 273–281). New York: Springer.
Kingdom, F. A. (2003). Color brings relief to human vision. Nature Neuroscience, 6 (6), 641–644.
Knill, D., & Richards, W. (1996). Perception as Bayesian inference. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kraft, J. M., & Brainard, D. H. (1999). Mechanisms of color constancy under nearly natural viewing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96 (1), 307–312.
Lekner, J., & Dorf, M. C. (1988). Why some things are darker when wet. Applied Optics, 27 (7), 1278–1280.
Lenaerts, T., Adams, B., & Dutré, P. (2008). Porous flow in particle-based fluid simulations. ACM Transactions on Graphics (TOG), 27 (3), 49:1–49:8.
Lu, J., Georghiades, A. S., Rushmeier, H., Dorsey, J., & Xu, C. (2006). Synthesis of material drying history: Phenomenon modeling, transferring and rendering. Paper presented at the ACM SIGGRAPH 2006 Courses, Boston, MA.
Maloney, L. T., & Wandell, B. A. (1986). Color constancy: A method for recovering surface spectral reflectance. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 3 (1), 29–33.
Mollon, J. D. (1989). “Tho'she kneel'd in that place where they grew…” The uses and origins of primate colour vision. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146 (1), 21–38.
Motoyoshi, I., Nishida, S., Sharan, L., & Adelson, E. H. (2007). Image statistics and the perception of surface qualities. Nature, 447 (7141), 206–209.
Nayar, S. K., Krishnan, G., Grossberg, M. D., & Raskar, R. (2006). Fast separation of direct and global components of a scene using high frequency illumination. ACM Transactions on Graphics, 25 (3), 935–944.
Nishida, S. Y., Motoyoshi, I., & Maruya, K. (2011). Luminance-color interactions in surface gloss perception. Journal of Vision, 11 (11): 397, doi:10.1167/11.11.397. [Abstract]
Olmos, A., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2004). A biologically inspired algorithm for the recovery of shading and reflectance images. Perception, 33 (12), 1463–1473.
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10 (4), 437–442.
Razlighi, Q. R., & Kehtarnavaz, N. (2009). A comparison study of image spatial entropy. Proceedings of SPIE 7257, Visual Communications and Image Processing 2009, 72571X, 1–10.
Rungjiratananon, W., Kanamori, Y., & Nishita, T. (2012). Wetting effects in hair simulation. In Proceedings of Pacific graphics (pp. 319–328). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
Sawayama, M., & Kimura, E. (2015). Stain on texture: Perception of a dark spot having a blurred edge on textured backgrounds. Vision Research, 109, 209–220.
Scheffé, H. (1952). An analysis of variance for paired comparisons. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 47 (259), 381–400.
Sharan, L., Li, Y., Motoyoshi, I., Nishida, S. Y., & Adelson, E. H. (2008). Image statistics for surface reflectance perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 25 (4), 846–865.
Steward, J. M., & Cole, B. L. (1989). What do color vision defectives say about everyday tasks? Optometry & Vision Science, 66 (5), 288–295.
Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B Methodological, 58, 267–288.
Twomey, S. A., Bohren, C. F., & Mergenthaler, J. L. (1986). Reflectance and albedo differences between wet and dry surfaces. Applied Optics, 25 (3), 431–437.
Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E. P., & Adelson, E. H. (2002). Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nature Neuroscience, 5 (6), 598–604.
Wyszecki, G., & Stiles, W. S. (1982). Color science (Vol. 8). New York: Wiley.
Zaidi, Q., Spehar, B., & DeBonet, J. (1997). Color constancy in variegated scenes: Role of low-level mechanisms in discounting illumination changes. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 14 (10), 2608–2621.
Appendix A
Several optical factors contribute to producing the color and luminance changes when a surface is wetted. Some are related to multiple reflections or subsurface scattering (Path 6 in Figure 1), while others are related to direct reflection, such as the physical cause of index matching (Path 5 in Figure 1). 
To explore the contributions of different optical factors, we measured the color of a dry or wet brick (rough dielectric material), which was illuminated by a projector (Epson EH-TW6600W, Espon, Tokyo, Japan) from the same direction with a color-calibrated camera (Pointgrey Flea 3, Richmond, BC, Canada) using a half mirror (Figure A1a). The surface was slanted 20° to attain the condition without the specular reflection. We separated the direct component of the brick from the global one using high-frequency illumination (Nayar et al., 2006). In theory, the method should work when only two images are used. However, as mentioned in the paper, we used 25 images of the brick illuminated by different checkerboard illuminations (Figure A1b) and extracted the on-(light checker) and off-(dark checker) images of the brick from the sequences. The off-image mainly included the global component from neighboring activated regions, although there were weak direct and global components because of the brightness of the deactivated light source. In contrast, the on-image strongly included the direct component and also included the global component from neighboring regions. If the spatial ratio of the activated and deactivated pixels (i.e., the ratio of the light and dark checkers) and the brightness of the deactivated source are given, these on- and off-images can be transformed to the direct and global components by a linear transformation. By using this method, the global component image (Figure A1c, lower) should arise from indirect factors, including multiple interreflection, subsurface scattering, volumetric scattering, or translucency. In contrast, the direct component image (Figure A1c, middle) arises mainly from the direct reflection from the brick, which is affected by index matching. 
Results of the color analyses between the wet and dry objects are shown in Figure A2. They show that under the direct component condition the color saturation of the wet object was higher than that of the dry one (Figure A2a). Under the global component condition the saturation of the wet object was also higher than that of the dry one as a whole (Figure A2b). The results suggest that both index matching (Path 5) and multiple reflections (Path 6) contribute to the optical changes produced by wetting a brick surface. 
Figure A1
 
Separation of direct and global reflectance components to analyze optical factors producing the color and luminance changes. We measured the color of a dry or wet brick (rough dielectric material), which was illuminated with a projector from the same direction as a color-calibrated camera using a half mirror (a). The surface normal was slanted 20° from the illumination/viewing axis to reduce specular reflections. We separated the direct component of the brick from the global one using high-frequency illumination (b) and extracted the on-(light checker) and off-(dark checker) images of the brick from the sequences. From these images, the direct (c, middle) and global (c, lower) components were extraced. The direct image (c, middle) should arise only from the direct reflection of the brick. In contrast, the global component image (c, lower) should arise from interreflection, subsurface scattering, volumetric scattering, or translucency.
Figure A1
 
Separation of direct and global reflectance components to analyze optical factors producing the color and luminance changes. We measured the color of a dry or wet brick (rough dielectric material), which was illuminated with a projector from the same direction as a color-calibrated camera using a half mirror (a). The surface normal was slanted 20° from the illumination/viewing axis to reduce specular reflections. We separated the direct component of the brick from the global one using high-frequency illumination (b) and extracted the on-(light checker) and off-(dark checker) images of the brick from the sequences. From these images, the direct (c, middle) and global (c, lower) components were extraced. The direct image (c, middle) should arise only from the direct reflection of the brick. In contrast, the global component image (c, lower) should arise from interreflection, subsurface scattering, volumetric scattering, or translucency.
Figure A2
 
Luminance and color differences between the wet and dry images for the direct and global reflectance components. (a) For the direct component images, the difference in color or luminance between the wet and dry images within the region-of-interest (ROI) is plotted in the right graphs. There are a clear luminance reduction and a saturation increase. Most of the image changes of the direct component can be ascribed to index matching (i.e., a decrease in the refractive index difference between air and the brick due to the brick's having a water film). (b) The results for the global component condition. Under the global component condition, the saturation of the wet object was also higher than that of the dry one as a whole, but the amount of the increase was smaller than that under the direct component condition.
Figure A2
 
Luminance and color differences between the wet and dry images for the direct and global reflectance components. (a) For the direct component images, the difference in color or luminance between the wet and dry images within the region-of-interest (ROI) is plotted in the right graphs. There are a clear luminance reduction and a saturation increase. Most of the image changes of the direct component can be ascribed to index matching (i.e., a decrease in the refractive index difference between air and the brick due to the brick's having a water film). (b) The results for the global component condition. Under the global component condition, the saturation of the wet object was also higher than that of the dry one as a whole, but the amount of the increase was smaller than that under the direct component condition.
Appendix B
In Experiment 1a, the WET operator was effective for some textures but not for others. Some of the examples used in Experiment 1a are shown in Figures B1 and B2 with the mean rating averaged across observers. 
Figure B1a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure 1
 
Optical properties of a surface largely change before and after wetting. (a) Demonstration of the change in the appearance of a surface by wetting. (b–e) Schematic diagrams of optical properties of wet and dry surfaces. (b) Light paths on a rough-surfaced solid material (dry). (c) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water. (d) Light paths on a porous material (dry). (e) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water.
Figure 1
 
Optical properties of a surface largely change before and after wetting. (a) Demonstration of the change in the appearance of a surface by wetting. (b–e) Schematic diagrams of optical properties of wet and dry surfaces. (b) Light paths on a rough-surfaced solid material (dry). (c) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water. (d) Light paths on a porous material (dry). (e) Light paths on the same material after wetting with water.
Figure 2
 
Photographs of dry and wet materials used in the image analysis. The color and luminance of the surface images were measured and analyzed before and after wetting.
Figure 2
 
Photographs of dry and wet materials used in the image analysis. The color and luminance of the surface images were measured and analyzed before and after wetting.
Figure 3
 
Color and luminance changes between the wet and dry surfaces. (a) The mean uv′ saturation (left) and the mean hue (right) of each wet surface are plotted as a function of those of each dry surface. The uv′ saturation and the hue were calculated using Equations 1 and 2, respectively. The mean hue was calculated using circular statistics (Equation 14). Results show that wetting a surface increases the saturation but has little effect on the hue. (b) The luminance statistics of each wet surface is plotted as a function of that of each dry surface. Different panels indicate different statistics as in the legend. The green line shows the relationship that there are no changes in the statistics between dry and wet surfaces. Results show that the mean luminance of the wet surface is lower than that of the dry surface. In addition, the skew of the luminance distribution tends to increase for wet surfaces in comparison with dry ones.
Figure 3
 
Color and luminance changes between the wet and dry surfaces. (a) The mean uv′ saturation (left) and the mean hue (right) of each wet surface are plotted as a function of those of each dry surface. The uv′ saturation and the hue were calculated using Equations 1 and 2, respectively. The mean hue was calculated using circular statistics (Equation 14). Results show that wetting a surface increases the saturation but has little effect on the hue. (b) The luminance statistics of each wet surface is plotted as a function of that of each dry surface. Different panels indicate different statistics as in the legend. The green line shows the relationship that there are no changes in the statistics between dry and wet surfaces. Results show that the mean luminance of the wet surface is lower than that of the dry surface. In addition, the skew of the luminance distribution tends to increase for wet surfaces in comparison with dry ones.
Figure 4
 
The color distribution of wet or dry surfaces in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates is shown in each panel. Different dots in the figure indicate different pixels of individual surfaces. The chromaticity values of all 32 surfaces are plotted in each figure. The white circle indicates the white point defined by the white checker in the Macbeth Color Checker. For both dry and wet surfaces, the mean chromaticity across all surfaces was close to the white point. The mean saturation across all materials in the wet condition, shown as the green line in each panel, was 1.43 times larger than that in the dry condition.
Figure 4
 
The color distribution of wet or dry surfaces in the uv′ chromaticity coordinates is shown in each panel. Different dots in the figure indicate different pixels of individual surfaces. The chromaticity values of all 32 surfaces are plotted in each figure. The white circle indicates the white point defined by the white checker in the Macbeth Color Checker. For both dry and wet surfaces, the mean chromaticity across all surfaces was close to the white point. The mean saturation across all materials in the wet condition, shown as the green line in each panel, was 1.43 times larger than that in the dry condition.
Figure 5
 
Effects of specular highlights on luminance distributions of a wet surface. By using a half mirror, a dry or wet material (a brick) was illuminated by a projector from the same direction that images were taken with the color-calibrated camera (see also Appendix A for the detailed camera setting.) (a) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was parallel to the illumination/camera direction (Slant = 0°). In this condition, the specular reflection from the wet surface directly returns to the camera because the illumination direction was the same as the camera direction. The luminance distribution of the wet surface was more positively skewed than that of the dry surface. (b) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was slanted 20°, where the specular reflection was almost completely excluded. In this case, surface wetting simply darkened most of the pixels, making the luminance histogram slightly skewed in the negative direction.
Figure 5
 
Effects of specular highlights on luminance distributions of a wet surface. By using a half mirror, a dry or wet material (a brick) was illuminated by a projector from the same direction that images were taken with the color-calibrated camera (see also Appendix A for the detailed camera setting.) (a) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was parallel to the illumination/camera direction (Slant = 0°). In this condition, the specular reflection from the wet surface directly returns to the camera because the illumination direction was the same as the camera direction. The luminance distribution of the wet surface was more positively skewed than that of the dry surface. (b) The pixel luminance on the wet surface is plotted as a function of that on the dry surface under the condition that the surface normal was slanted 20°, where the specular reflection was almost completely excluded. In this case, surface wetting simply darkened most of the pixels, making the luminance histogram slightly skewed in the negative direction.
Figure 6
 
Demonstration of the WET (wetness enhancing transformation) operator. The right image was produced from the left one by i) enhancing the saturation of pixel colors, and ii) tone-remapping of luminance signals with an accelerating nonlinear function, which makes the intensity histogram positively skewed, and decreases the mean luminance. This WET operator tends to make a dry surface look wetter.
Figure 6
 
Demonstration of the WET (wetness enhancing transformation) operator. The right image was produced from the left one by i) enhancing the saturation of pixel colors, and ii) tone-remapping of luminance signals with an accelerating nonlinear function, which makes the intensity histogram positively skewed, and decreases the mean luminance. This WET operator tends to make a dry surface look wetter.
Figure 7
 
The WET operator tends to make the original image look wetter, while the effect size varies depending on the image. (a) Stimulus examples of the original and the transformed images. The figure below each stimulus shows the hue histogram of the stimulus. The effect of the WET operator was strong for the upper two examples but weak for the lower one. (b) Results of the wetness-rating experiment. The horizontal axis indicates the rating for the original image, and the vertical axis indicates the rating for the transformed image. Each symbol indicates individual images. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 7
 
The WET operator tends to make the original image look wetter, while the effect size varies depending on the image. (a) Stimulus examples of the original and the transformed images. The figure below each stimulus shows the hue histogram of the stimulus. The effect of the WET operator was strong for the upper two examples but weak for the lower one. (b) Results of the wetness-rating experiment. The horizontal axis indicates the rating for the original image, and the vertical axis indicates the rating for the transformed image. Each symbol indicates individual images. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 8
 
Contributions of color saturation and luminance distribution skew to wet perception. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 1b, where we continuously and independently manipulated the color saturation and the luminance histogram skew of a natural surface using histogram matching. (b) Results of Experiment 1b. The horizontal axis indicates the mean saturation defined by Equation 1, and the vertical axis indicates the skew of the luminance histogram. The size of the red circles in each panel indicates the mean rating averaged across observers. The width of the blue contour in each panel indicates + 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that both saturation and luminance skew contribute to enhancing the wetness impression by the WET operator. (c) Separate plots for the effects of the luminance skew and the color saturation. In the left panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across color saturation conditions are plotted as a function of the luminance skew. In the right panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across the luminance skew conditions are plotted as a function of the color saturation. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 8
 
Contributions of color saturation and luminance distribution skew to wet perception. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 1b, where we continuously and independently manipulated the color saturation and the luminance histogram skew of a natural surface using histogram matching. (b) Results of Experiment 1b. The horizontal axis indicates the mean saturation defined by Equation 1, and the vertical axis indicates the skew of the luminance histogram. The size of the red circles in each panel indicates the mean rating averaged across observers. The width of the blue contour in each panel indicates + 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that both saturation and luminance skew contribute to enhancing the wetness impression by the WET operator. (c) Separate plots for the effects of the luminance skew and the color saturation. In the left panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across color saturation conditions are plotted as a function of the luminance skew. In the right panel, the mean wetness ratings averaged across the luminance skew conditions are plotted as a function of the color saturation. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers.
Figure 9
 
The effect of chroma, another definition of the color saturation. For the two images used in Experiment 1a (Surface 1 and 2), we continuously modulated the uv′ saturation and measured the perceived wetness. The results are plotted as a function of the mean chroma of each stimulus. The vertical orange line indicates the mean chroma of the original image. The horizontal green line indicates the mean rating in Experiment 1a for the original image. Different symbols in each panel indicate different skew conditions. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that the wetting impression increases with color saturation even when chroma is used as the definition of saturation.
Figure 9
 
The effect of chroma, another definition of the color saturation. For the two images used in Experiment 1a (Surface 1 and 2), we continuously modulated the uv′ saturation and measured the perceived wetness. The results are plotted as a function of the mean chroma of each stimulus. The vertical orange line indicates the mean chroma of the original image. The horizontal green line indicates the mean rating in Experiment 1a for the original image. Different symbols in each panel indicate different skew conditions. Error bars indicate ± 1 SEM across observers. The results indicate that the wetting impression increases with color saturation even when chroma is used as the definition of saturation.
Figure 10
 
Regression analysis as to which image features contribute to the effect size of WET. (a) Results of the single regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant correlations with the effect size (p < 0.05). (a) Results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant weights (p < 0.05). The results indicate significant contributions of luminance histogram skew, luminance spatial entropy, hue (global) entropy, and hue spatial entropy. (b) Results of the lasso regression analysis. The results support strong positive contributions of hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy.
Figure 10
 
Regression analysis as to which image features contribute to the effect size of WET. (a) Results of the single regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant correlations with the effect size (p < 0.05). (a) Results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis. Red characters indicate that these predictor variables had statistically significant weights (p < 0.05). The results indicate significant contributions of luminance histogram skew, luminance spatial entropy, hue (global) entropy, and hue spatial entropy. (b) Results of the lasso regression analysis. The results support strong positive contributions of hue entropy and luminance spatial entropy.
Figure 11
 
Effects of the WET operator depend on the hue entropy. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 2. We used a physically based renderer to make the original images. By modulating the peak in reflectance spectra of each object, we controlled the hue entropy of the image. The hue entropy of each image under the small, middle, or large hue entropy conditions was 2.8, 4.8, or 6.6, respectively. To make the transformed image, we applied the WET operator to the original image. (b) Results of the wetness judgment (left) and darkness judgment (right). Ratings of the pairwise comparison were converted into a one-dimensional perceptual scale. The results indicate that the wetness perception of a scene image increases with the hue entropy of the image without changes in the luminance spatial entropy or in other factors.
Figure 11
 
Effects of the WET operator depend on the hue entropy. (a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 2. We used a physically based renderer to make the original images. By modulating the peak in reflectance spectra of each object, we controlled the hue entropy of the image. The hue entropy of each image under the small, middle, or large hue entropy conditions was 2.8, 4.8, or 6.6, respectively. To make the transformed image, we applied the WET operator to the original image. (b) Results of the wetness judgment (left) and darkness judgment (right). Ratings of the pairwise comparison were converted into a one-dimensional perceptual scale. The results indicate that the wetness perception of a scene image increases with the hue entropy of the image without changes in the luminance spatial entropy or in other factors.
Figure 12
 
Two-dimensional (2-D) Mondrian patterns used in a preliminary test of Experiment 2. Some observers showed results similar to those obtained with 3-D patterns, but others had trouble judging wetness for the 2-D patterns.
Figure 12
 
Two-dimensional (2-D) Mondrian patterns used in a preliminary test of Experiment 2. Some observers showed results similar to those obtained with 3-D patterns, but others had trouble judging wetness for the 2-D patterns.
Figure 13
 
(a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 3. (b) Results of Experiment 3. The results indicate that decreasing the mean luminance (green) is not sufficient for significantly increasing the wetting impression.
Figure 13
 
(a) Stimulus examples used in Experiment 3. (b) Results of Experiment 3. The results indicate that decreasing the mean luminance (green) is not sufficient for significantly increasing the wetting impression.
Figure 14
 
Hue entropy can reduce the ambiguity of physical events. (a) Schematic explanations of the relationship between physical events and image features. Besides surface wetness, other factors, such as the existence of highly color-saturated glossy surfaces, can produce similar image features. This is why the optical image features of luminance and saturation are not sufficient for estimating that the surface is wet. (b) Explanation of a framework that can explain the effects of hue entropy. Each colored panel indicates the probabilities of the occurrence of a wet or dry scene. As in Figure 4, we here assume that the probabilities of the occurrence for wet colors spread 1.43 times more widely than that for dry colors. The upper and lower panels indicate the small and large hue entropy conditions, respectively. For the large hue entropy, the posterior probability for the wet scene can be larger than that for the dry scene, while it tends to be smaller for the small hue entropy.
Figure 14
 
Hue entropy can reduce the ambiguity of physical events. (a) Schematic explanations of the relationship between physical events and image features. Besides surface wetness, other factors, such as the existence of highly color-saturated glossy surfaces, can produce similar image features. This is why the optical image features of luminance and saturation are not sufficient for estimating that the surface is wet. (b) Explanation of a framework that can explain the effects of hue entropy. Each colored panel indicates the probabilities of the occurrence of a wet or dry scene. As in Figure 4, we here assume that the probabilities of the occurrence for wet colors spread 1.43 times more widely than that for dry colors. The upper and lower panels indicate the small and large hue entropy conditions, respectively. For the large hue entropy, the posterior probability for the wet scene can be larger than that for the dry scene, while it tends to be smaller for the small hue entropy.
Figure 15
 
The results of a simulation of the model in Figure 14 under an assumption that the probability distribution of color is approximated by a Gaussian function, with the spread's being ×1.43 wider for the wet condition than for the dry condition. A diagonal view (a) and a top view (b) of the frequency distribution histogram of the combination of ΠP(Sx|Dry) and ΠP(Sx|Wet). The blue histogram was obtained when N color samples were randomly taken from the wet color distribution; the red histogram was obtained when they were randomly taken from the dry color distribution. The dotted line is the boundary above which the likelihood is higher for wet than for dry. When the number of samples is one (N = 1), blue and red histograms significantly overlap each other. As the number of samples is increased, however, the blue and red distributions are gradually separated from each other, making it easier to tell whether the color samples are from the wet or dry distribution.
Figure 15
 
The results of a simulation of the model in Figure 14 under an assumption that the probability distribution of color is approximated by a Gaussian function, with the spread's being ×1.43 wider for the wet condition than for the dry condition. A diagonal view (a) and a top view (b) of the frequency distribution histogram of the combination of ΠP(Sx|Dry) and ΠP(Sx|Wet). The blue histogram was obtained when N color samples were randomly taken from the wet color distribution; the red histogram was obtained when they were randomly taken from the dry color distribution. The dotted line is the boundary above which the likelihood is higher for wet than for dry. When the number of samples is one (N = 1), blue and red histograms significantly overlap each other. As the number of samples is increased, however, the blue and red distributions are gradually separated from each other, making it easier to tell whether the color samples are from the wet or dry distribution.
Figure 16
 
Examples of the DET operator. The DET can tend to make the original images look dryer.
Figure 16
 
Examples of the DET operator. The DET can tend to make the original images look dryer.
Figure A1
 
Separation of direct and global reflectance components to analyze optical factors producing the color and luminance changes. We measured the color of a dry or wet brick (rough dielectric material), which was illuminated with a projector from the same direction as a color-calibrated camera using a half mirror (a). The surface normal was slanted 20° from the illumination/viewing axis to reduce specular reflections. We separated the direct component of the brick from the global one using high-frequency illumination (b) and extracted the on-(light checker) and off-(dark checker) images of the brick from the sequences. From these images, the direct (c, middle) and global (c, lower) components were extraced. The direct image (c, middle) should arise only from the direct reflection of the brick. In contrast, the global component image (c, lower) should arise from interreflection, subsurface scattering, volumetric scattering, or translucency.
Figure A1
 
Separation of direct and global reflectance components to analyze optical factors producing the color and luminance changes. We measured the color of a dry or wet brick (rough dielectric material), which was illuminated with a projector from the same direction as a color-calibrated camera using a half mirror (a). The surface normal was slanted 20° from the illumination/viewing axis to reduce specular reflections. We separated the direct component of the brick from the global one using high-frequency illumination (b) and extracted the on-(light checker) and off-(dark checker) images of the brick from the sequences. From these images, the direct (c, middle) and global (c, lower) components were extraced. The direct image (c, middle) should arise only from the direct reflection of the brick. In contrast, the global component image (c, lower) should arise from interreflection, subsurface scattering, volumetric scattering, or translucency.
Figure A2
 
Luminance and color differences between the wet and dry images for the direct and global reflectance components. (a) For the direct component images, the difference in color or luminance between the wet and dry images within the region-of-interest (ROI) is plotted in the right graphs. There are a clear luminance reduction and a saturation increase. Most of the image changes of the direct component can be ascribed to index matching (i.e., a decrease in the refractive index difference between air and the brick due to the brick's having a water film). (b) The results for the global component condition. Under the global component condition, the saturation of the wet object was also higher than that of the dry one as a whole, but the amount of the increase was smaller than that under the direct component condition.
Figure A2
 
Luminance and color differences between the wet and dry images for the direct and global reflectance components. (a) For the direct component images, the difference in color or luminance between the wet and dry images within the region-of-interest (ROI) is plotted in the right graphs. There are a clear luminance reduction and a saturation increase. Most of the image changes of the direct component can be ascribed to index matching (i.e., a decrease in the refractive index difference between air and the brick due to the brick's having a water film). (b) The results for the global component condition. Under the global component condition, the saturation of the wet object was also higher than that of the dry one as a whole, but the amount of the increase was smaller than that under the direct component condition.
Figure B1a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B1b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was effective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2a
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
Figure B2b
 
Stimulus examples on which the WET operator was ineffective in Experiment 1a.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×