Open Access
Article  |   June 2017
Measuring nonlinear signal combination using EEG
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision June 2017, Vol.17, 10. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/17.5.10
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Darren G. M. Cunningham, Daniel H. Baker, Jonathan W. Peirce; Measuring nonlinear signal combination using EEG. Journal of Vision 2017;17(5):10. https://doi.org/10.1167/17.5.10.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Relatively little is known about the processes, both linear and nonlinear, by which signals are combined beyond V1. By presenting two stimulus components simultaneously, flickering at different temporal frequencies (frequency tagging) while measuring steady-state visual evoked potentials, we can assess responses to the individual components, including direct measurements of suppression on each other, and various nonlinear responses to their combination found at intermodulation frequencies. The result is a rather rich dataset of frequencies at which responses can be found. We presented pairs of sinusoidal gratings at different temporal frequencies, forming plaid patterns that were “coherent” (looking like a checkerboard) and “noncoherent” (looking like a pair of transparently overlaid gratings), and found clear intermodulation responses to compound stimuli, indicating nonlinear summation. This might have been attributed to cross-orientation suppression except that the pattern of intermodulation responses differed for coherent and noncoherent patterns, whereas the effects of suppression (measured at the component frequencies) did not. A two-stage model of nonlinear summation involving conjunction detection with a logical AND gate described the data well, capturing the difference between coherent and noncoherent plaids over a wide array of possible response frequencies. Multistimulus frequency-tagged EEG in combination with computational modeling may be a very valuable tool in studying the conjunction of these signals. In the current study the results suggest a second-order mechanism responding selectively to coherent plaid patterns.

Introduction
Relatively little is known about how the visual system encodes representations beyond oriented bars and edges in V1 (for a review, see Peirce, 2015). We do know that the visual system is nonlinear in various ways. It uses suppressive nonlinear operations to normalize response properties (e.g., Carandini & Heeger, 2012), which might serve to reduce redundancy in the neural representation of natural scenes (e.g., Schwartz & Simoncelli, 2001). Nonlinear processes are also necessary to perform logical AND operations driving selective responses to particular stimulus combinations (Gheorghiu & Kingdom, 2009; Peirce, 2007a, 2011). These might be useful in detecting various features such as a curved contour (the AND operation of at least two neurons with abutting receptive fields and slightly different orientations: Gheorghiu & Kingdom, 2009; Hancock & Peirce, 2008) or coherent plaid (the AND operation of two neurons with spatially overlapping receptive fields but different tuning). 
The existence of multiple nonlinearities in response to compound stimuli poses the methodological problem of having to disentangle them for individual examination. Psychophysical approaches and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measure the summed nonlinear response to stimuli, making them unsuitable for this particular task. Electroencephalography (EEG) responses, however, may be more applicable. Responses to sensory stimuli can be measured by frequency tagging a stimulus, temporally modulating its intensity at a fixed rate and measuring the entrained neural response at the same temporal frequency. Multiple stimuli can be tagged simultaneously by using a different temporal frequency for each stimulus (for a review, see Norcia, Appelbaum, Ales, Cottereau, & Rossion, 2015; Regan, 1983 Regan & Heron, 1969; Regan & Regam, 1988; Spekreijse & Reits, 1982; Zemon & Ratliff, 1984). We can, for example, present stimuli at 5 and 7 Hz, allowing us to measure independently the response to each stimulus at 5 Hz (ƒ1) and 7 Hz (ƒ2), and thus examine the effect of one stimulus on the response to another by masking, for example. 
Furthermore, intermodulation responses at the difference (ƒ2 − ƒ1: 2 Hz) and sum (ƒ1 + ƒ2: 12 Hz) of these fundamental frequencies can also be measured. These, and their harmonics, indicate a nonlinearity at or after the point of summation (for a mathematical explanation, see Regan & Regan, 1988) although, on their own, intermodulation responses do not tell us what mechanism may have caused them (e.g., a suppressive mechanism or an AND gate). 
Several recent studies have employed measurements of responses at intermodulation frequencies to examine signal combinations in visual cortex. Boremanse, Norcia, and Rossion (2013) presented half-face stimuli at two different frequencies and measured the sum and difference intermodulation responses. These were larger when two face halves were presented as a whole face compared to when they were separated vertically or horizontally. They attribute this to a nonlinear mechanism sensitive to the whole, rather than the parts, of the face. Similarly, intermodulation responses have been found when parts of a Kanizsa-type illusory stimulus (Kanizsa, 1979) are presented at different rates (Alp, Kogo, Van Belle, Wagemans, & Rossion, 2016; Gundlach & Müller, 2013), when a texture is distinct from its background in a figure–ground experiment (Appelbaum, Wade, Pettet, Vildavski, & Norcia, 2008) and when the abutting sides of a square are moved closer together during perceptual form-motion binding (Aissani, Cottereau, Dumas, Paradis, & Lorenceau, 2011). 
Intermodulation responses have also been used to study normalization processes using grating and plaid stimuli. Tsai, Wade, and Norcia (2012) found that the sum intermodulation term amplitude peaked when a test pattern was masked by a pattern of equivalent contrast but disappeared when contrasts were different. Also, Candy, Skoczenski, and Norcia (2001) and Baker, Norcia, and Candy (2011) presented pairs of oriented gratings at different temporal frequencies and found intermodulation responses when they were parallel in orientation. In both cases, intermodulation responses can be explained as suppressive interactions between units. 
The nonlinearity giving rise to the intermodulation response beyond these early normalization processes may also involve an additional nonlinear mechanism sensitive to the compound stimulus of two gratings, an AND gate responding to plaids. Particular combinations of gratings form the “coherent” percept of a distinct checkerboard pattern, while other “noncoherent” combinations appear more readily as two semitransparent, superimposed gratings (Adelson & Movshon, 1982; Meese & Freeman, 1995). Contrast adaptation has been found to plaids above and beyond that expected by adaptation to the components (McGovern & Peirce, 2007; Peirce & Taylor, 2006; Robinson & MacLeod, 2011), but only when the spatial frequency of the components was matched (Hancock, McGovern, & Peirce, 2010). Similarly, a visual search task has suggested pre-attentive mechanisms sensitive only to coherent plaid patterns (Nam, Solomon, Morgan, Wright, & Chubb, 2009). 
This dependence on coherence for the putative plaid detector presents a possible way to distinguish the nonlinearities feeding the intermodulation response. If special mechanisms for plaids exist and are partially driving the intermodulation responses, then we would expect such intermodulation responses to differ according to the coherence. Conversely if the intermodulation response is driven by a normalization pool for cross-orientation suppression (XOS), which is thought to be relatively untuned to spatial frequency (DeAngelis, Freeman, & Ohzawa, 1994; Petrov, Carandini, & McKee, 2005), then the intermodulation response should be similar for different forms of plaid. 
Here, we compare intermodulation responses to a range of grating combinations, including both coherent and noncoherent plaid patterns. To create noncoherent patterns we used substantially different spatial frequencies (1 and 3 cpd for the two components) that have previously been found to result in clear noncoherent patterns in moving stimuli (Adelson & Movshon, 1982). If the intermodulation responses are caused only by suppressive mechanisms, we would expect to find it in both forms of compound pattern. Conversely, if the intermodulation response results both from suppressive mechanisms and mechanisms sensitive to plaid patterns then we would expect to find larger intermodulation responses to coherent plaids but similar levels of component suppression for both coherent and noncoherent plaids. We generated a multilayer network model with XOS in Layer 1 and a combinatorial AND gate in Layer 2 and found that it described the data well. 
Methods
Participants
Fifteen participants (seven females, eight males) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision gave informed consent to participate in the study. The ethics board at the School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, granted ethical approval. The work was conducted in accordance with the 2008 version of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Apparatus and materials
A computer-controlled LCD monitor (Iiyama Prolite X2472HD, Iiyama, Hoofddorp, The Netherlands) with a screen resolution of 1920 × 1080 pixels, mean luminance of 133.17 cd/m2, and a refresh rate of 60 Hz was used for stimulus presentation. The gamma function of the screen was linearized using a photometer (ColorCal MKII, Cambridge Research Systems, Kent, UK). Participants sat at a screen distance of approximately 100−120 cm, depending on how they felt comfortable; this was measured for each participant and used to maintain a constant stimulus size measured in degrees of visual angle. The PsychoPy stimulus generation library (Peirce, 2007b) was used for stimulus presentation and collecting participant responses to a simple detection task. A button box (Cedrus RB-830 Response Pad, Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA) was used for participants to make these responses. 
EEG data acquisition
A DBPA-1 Sensorium bio-amplifier (Sensorium Inc., Charlotte, VT) was used for EEG recording at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. Voltage responses were recorded from 122 electrode channels (silver/silver-chloride) on a set of customized whole-head caps with twisted and fixed electrode cables (EasyCap, Munich, Germany). This included a ground electrode placed on the forehead, reference electrode at the left mastoid, EOG electrodes (RHE, LHE, and LIO), and 117 scalp electrodes. Caps were centered on electrode Cz, halfway between the nasion and inion. Where possible, impedances were brought below 25 kilohms (kΩ) before the experiment began (below 50 kΩ if this could not be achieved). Note that on many commercial systems this might seem like high impedance, but is the suggested setting for the Sensorium DBPA-1 amplifier. 
A parallel port from the stimulus computer was used to indicate when a stimulus onset occurred, sending a trigger signal time-locked to the screen refresh. We confirmed in advance that this was precise on our hardware, using a photometer connected to the amplifier via a StimTracker (Cedrus Light Sensor, Cedrus Corporation). 
Stimuli and experimental procedure
Stimuli comprised of two sinusoidal gratings (denoted as “A” and “B”) and various combinations thereof. Grating A had a spatial frequency of 1 cpd and Grating B a spatial frequency of 3 cpd. These could then be combined with a second, spatially orthogonal grating to form plaid patterns that were either coherent (“AA,” “BB”) or noncoherent (“AB,” “BA”) as shown in Figure 1. On each trial the overall orientation of the stimulus, either grating or plaid, was randomly assigned, but the orthogonal configuration of the grating components that formed a plaid was maintained. 
Figure 1
 
Grating/plaid examples. Left: Grating component alone examples. Middle: Coherent plaid examples. Note that these always look like checkerboards; one cannot “choose” to perceive them as a pair of gratings. Right: Noncoherent plaid examples in which the pattern can be perceived as a pair of gratings without any automatic fusing as a single checkerboard pattern. The top row represents configurations for the 1 cpd components, and the bottom row the 3 cpd components, although these are only for illustrative purposes rather than being drawn to scale.
Figure 1
 
Grating/plaid examples. Left: Grating component alone examples. Middle: Coherent plaid examples. Note that these always look like checkerboards; one cannot “choose” to perceive them as a pair of gratings. Right: Noncoherent plaid examples in which the pattern can be perceived as a pair of gratings without any automatic fusing as a single checkerboard pattern. The top row represents configurations for the 1 cpd components, and the bottom row the 3 cpd components, although these are only for illustrative purposes rather than being drawn to scale.
Components were presented within a 7.5°-diameter circular window with a raised cosine edge profile (width = 1.5°). In the center of the stimulus a further circle (diameter 0.4°) was placed with the same midgray color as in the background, also with a raised cosine edge profile (0.08°). This was done to accentuate the fixation dot, a red circle subtending 0.175° in the center of the screen. A further green dot (0.25° diameter) appeared occasionally and briefly, located at a radius of 3.75° from the centrally located fixation point but a random radial angle. This was used as part of the attentional control task that participants were asked to perform. 
When seated in the recording booth, participants were instructed to maintain fixation on the central red fixation point. They had to conduct an attentional task independent of the stimulus presentation to ensure that attention was constant, a practice common in fMRI studies since the finding that differential attention to different stimuli can provide an important confound (Huk, Ress, & Heeger, 2001). In our attentional control task participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible when they detected the appearance of the green task dot (without looking towards it) by pressing the central button on the response box. This green dot appeared at a random time within any trial number divisible by 10 or 6 (6, 10, 12, 18, etc.). This resulted in rare events, unpredictable to the participants, that required their attention to detect, preventing them from assigning greater or lesser attention to the actual stimulus. Although diverting attention away from the stimulus might decrease our chances of measuring strong signals (Appelbaum et al., 2009) we considered it more important to keep attention constant. It remains very possible, however, that we underestimate the size of responses in our data as a result of this. The percentage of task occurrences across each condition relative to the grand total number of occurrences shows no bias towards any particular condition (A1: 13.33%; A2: 13.07%; B1: 14.67%; B2: 10.67%; A1A2: 12.27%; B1B2: 13.33%; A1B2: 9.60%; B1A2: 13.07%). It is therefore unlikely that the dot task was a factor in determining any systematic effects observed between conditions. Participants responded to 76.34% (SEM = 2.22) of dot occurrences, indicating that the task was neither too easy nor too difficult. 
A brief presentation of the experiment was provided to ensure the participants understood what they were being asked to do. Following this, they were presented with a blank fixation screen consisting of a gray background and red fixation dot until they indicated that they were ready to begin. 
To generate the intermodulation responses in plaid conditions, the two grating components had to have their contrast intensity simultaneously modulated at slightly different temporal frequencies. The frequencies chosen to do so were 2.3 and 3.75 Hz on the basis that they allowed the response frequencies to be temporally incommensurate; the fundamental frequencies (2.3, 3.75 Hz), their harmonics (2ƒ1: 4.6 Hz, 2ƒ2: 7.5 Hz) and the intermodulation terms (ƒ2 − ƒ1: 1.45 Hz, ƒ1 + ƒ2: 6.05 Hz) and harmonics (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1: 2.9 Hz, 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 12.1 Hz) would not overlap. Further, we aimed to avoid overlap between the typical alpha band (8–12 Hz) and the sum intermodulation response frequency (ƒ1 + ƒ2: 6.05 Hz) as in Boremanse et al. (2013) to boost signal to noise. 
The contrast of any one component varied sinusoidally in time between 0% and 50% of the maximal contrast of the monitor (which had a maximal Michelson contrast of 0.99). The plaid patterns were formed from the signed sum (black is negative, white is positive) of the pixel values resulting from the two components such that, when the components were both at the peak of their sinusoidal modulation in time, a standard plaid pattern was physically present momentarily, whereas when either component was at its trough only the other component was physically present, as a simple grating. Due, presumably, to the reasonably high rate of contrast modulation the percept for the observer is not of two gratings changing contrast gradually. Rather the percept is still of a pattern that alternates with gratings at a high and unpredictable rate. Movie 1 shows what a coherent plaid condition (A1A2) looked like. 
For the remainder of this article, “1” denotes when a grating component was flickered at 2.3 Hz and “2” when flickered at 3.75 Hz. For example, Grating A flickered at 2.3 Hz will now be referred to as A1, while a coherent plaid with Grating A components will be referred to as A1A2, where one component was flickered at 2.3 Hz and the other at 3.75 Hz. This resulted in eight stimuli: four grating components (A1, A2, B1, and B2), two coherent plaids (A1A2 and B1B2), and two noncoherent plaids (A1B2 and B1A2). A trial consisted of an 11-s presentation of a flickering grating or two simultaneously flickering, superimposed grating components, followed by a 7–9-s interstimulus interval. Each of the eight stimuli was presented three times in a run, with five runs in total. Participants were given short breaks between runs. Upon completion, participants were thanked, debriefed, and given the opportunity to ask any questions. 
Analytical procedure
Data were band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 100 Hz. They were then epoched according to stimulus onset, with the first second of data removed to exclude onset transients from the analysis, resulting in a 10-s epoch for each trial. Trials were time averaged by condition for each participant, averaging out activity that is not phase-locked to the stimulus presentation, such as the alpha-wave response. Fast Fourier transforms (FFTs) were then conducted on these average waveforms to bring the data into frequency space, resulting in amplitude responses (μV) at discrete frequencies (for a 10-s stimulus the FFT has a resolution of 0.1 Hz) between 0.1 and 100 Hz. 
The amplitude response at each frequency at each electrode site was converted into a measure of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) by dividing the amplitude at the frequency of interest by the average amplitude of the surrounding 12 frequency bins. The data used in the following analyses were taken from electrode Oz. This was based on group topographies showing peak responses for all stimuli occurring there, and is consistent with measurements in the vicinity of primary visual cortex. We also conducted the analysis using a cluster of electrodes around Oz, but this made no difference to the conclusions from the analyses. 
Initially, the frequencies of interest for analysis were at ƒ1 (2.3 Hz), ƒ2 (3.75 Hz), ƒ2 − ƒ1 (1.45 Hz), and ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz). Upon inspection of the data, responses at the 2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 (2.9 Hz) and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz) frequencies warranted further analysis. To determine whether SNRs were significantly above background noise (SNR = 1), a series of one-sample t tests were conducted separately for each set of component SNRs (i.e., A1, A1A2, and A1B2; A2, A1A2, and B1A2; B1, B1B2, and B1A2; B2, B1B2, and A1B2), difference intermodulation SNRs (at ƒ2 − ƒ1 and 2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1: A1, A2, B2, A1A2, and A1B2), and sum intermodulation SNRs (at ƒ1 + ƒ2 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: A1, A2, B2, A1A2, and A1B2). Each series of t tests were corrected using the ranked Bonferroni-Holm method to control for Type 1 errors (Holm, 1979). 
The extent to which grating/plaid pattern predicted response SNRs was examined using linear mixed-effects modeling. The analytical model was generated using the mixed function of the Afex package in R (Singmann et al., 2016). Grating/plaid pattern was the only predictor with random slopes as a function of “participant” using a maximal random effects structure (as recommended by Barr et al., 2013). This model was applied to both component SNRs and intermodulation SNRs. The lsmeans function in R for examining pairwise comparisons from linear mixed-effects model structures was used when a significant main effect of pattern was found, and comparisons were corrected using the Tukey honest significance difference method for multiple comparisons (Russell, 2016). 
Modeling
A two-layer network model was generated based on Peirce (2007a, 2011; see Figure 2) in which static sigmoidal nonlinearities were applied to the outputs of each channel (as expected by any model of V1 outputs) and simply summed by a “Layer 2” mechanism, which also has a sigmoidal nonlinearity. Such a model can be used to investigate the relationship between XOS, nonlinear additive summation, and the generation of intermodulation responses to plaids. Contrast input to the model was generated in the same way that contrast was modulated in the stimuli presented to participants:  where Display FormulaImage not available represents a point in time between 0 and 11 s (in steps of 0.01 s), Display FormulaImage not available the temporal frequency for that component, and Display FormulaImage not available the maximum Michelson contrast (set to 0.5). The multiplication and addition by 0.5 scaled the minimum and maximum contrast to be 0 and 1 initially and the further multiplication by Display FormulaImage not available reduced the maximum contrast to 0.5.  
Figure 2
 
Model schematic displaying how contrast input was transformed within each layer. Though the Layer 1 channels were untuned in this study, the Gabor patches differing in orientation and spatial frequency are visual representations used to indicate how the channels, within the framework that we propose, might be tuned. These are followed by a static sigmoidal nonlinearity and then a temporal filter (if appropriate to the simulation). The normalization pool exerted its influence (for XOS) as part of the static nonlinearity. A similar configuration (without normalization) was used to transform input into Layer 2 channels.
Figure 2
 
Model schematic displaying how contrast input was transformed within each layer. Though the Layer 1 channels were untuned in this study, the Gabor patches differing in orientation and spatial frequency are visual representations used to indicate how the channels, within the framework that we propose, might be tuned. These are followed by a static sigmoidal nonlinearity and then a temporal filter (if appropriate to the simulation). The normalization pool exerted its influence (for XOS) as part of the static nonlinearity. A similar configuration (without normalization) was used to transform input into Layer 2 channels.
Layer 1 involved four channels with static nonlinearities (Naka & Rushton, 1966) with an extra term in their denominators to account for XOS (Carandini & Heeger, 2012) and is here assumed to represent grating component responses. The structure of a Layer 1 channel was as follows:  where Display FormulaImage not available is the maximum response of the channel, Display FormulaImage not available the input contrast of the stimulus component being encoded at that channel at time point Display FormulaImage not available, Display FormulaImage not available an exponent, and Display FormulaImage not availablethe semisaturation point. Display FormulaImage not available, used as the normalization pool, is the sum of three extra terms corresponding to the input to the other three Layer 1 channels. The value of each of these terms was determined in a similar fashion to the Display FormulaImage not available term:    
The Display FormulaImage not available was held constant at 1, Display FormulaImage not available at 2, and Display FormulaImage not available at 0.2. Two channels corresponded to “detectors” for components A1 and A2, and their output was summed (Display FormulaImage not available) before being passed to Layer 2. The same was done for the other two channels, (B1 and B') and their sum referred to as Display FormulaImage not available.  
The second layer can be thought of as two additional “channels” with a similar static nonlinearity (without the additional XOS term on the denominator), which would respond selectively to the presence of a plaid:  where Display FormulaImage not available (either a or b) represents the linearly summed component responses (as described above), Display FormulaImage not available an exponent, and Display FormulaImage not availablethe semisaturation point (the same used in Layer 1). As for the Layer 1 channels, Display FormulaImage not available was held constant at 1, Display FormulaImage not available at 2, and Display FormulaImage not available at 0.2. The overall output of the model was then calculated as the linearly summed response of Display FormulaImage not available and Display FormulaImage not available, simulating the population response measured with EEG.  
It has been suggested that the temporal processing of signals by the mechanisms generating intermodulation responses may be key to their almost-always asymmetric response patterns (e.g., Alp et al., 2016; Boremanse et al., 2013). Thus, we wanted to evaluate the importance of neural impulse response functions (NIRs) on the performance of the model. Further, it has been suggested that later mechanisms in the visual pathway may differ in their temporal filtering properties compared to earlier mechanisms. We therefore conducted simulations where NIRs were generated and used as temporal filters on each channel's output. 
We tested two variants of filter (Figure 3). The first variant was generated by summing three Gaussian distributions. It had a bandpass Fourier response peaking at ∼5 Hz with a slow decay towards 20 Hz. The second form of linear filter was generated simply by halving the peaks and widths of the band-pass filter described above, resulting in a high(er)-pass Fourier response peaking at ∼10 Hz. These filters were combined in three ways: (a) the bandpass filter for both Layer 1 and Layer 2, (b) the bandpass filter for Layer 1 and higher frequency filter for Layer 2, and (c) no temporal filtering at either level. This allowed us to assess the importance of the temporal integration functions in the resulting responses, and whether there is any evidence for these differing in early- and late-stages of the model. 
Figure 3
 
Temporal and Fourier responses of the bandpass and higher pass temporal filters. The left figures represent temporal responses and the right Fourier responses. Red indicates responses of the bandpass filter and blue the higher pass filter.
Figure 3
 
Temporal and Fourier responses of the bandpass and higher pass temporal filters. The left figures represent temporal responses and the right Fourier responses. Red indicates responses of the bandpass filter and blue the higher pass filter.
Following this, noise was added to the output of the model. A vector of values randomly sampled from a normal distribution (N = 10,000) was filtered with a third order bandpass Butterworth filter between 0.1 and 100 Hz. This resulted in a 1/ƒ decay like that observed in the EEG data. The mean of the random distribution allowed us to scale the overall magnitude of the 1/ƒ noise and the SD the degree of randomization (M = 10 and SD = 0.5 for no NIR, M = 190 and SD = 9.5 for application of NIR). 
FFTs were performed on the model output with the added noise. The magnitude of model responses was normalized between 0 and 1 by using the minimum and maximum response for the condition being simulated. This controlled for the arbitrary scaling of model responses introduced by the temporal filter. In all cases, this maximum was found at the 1/f noise magnitude at 0.1 Hz, rather than at a signal bin related to the stimulus input. The same approach that was used for the EEG data was used to calculate SNRs at each frequency, and these were then used to perform model fitting. 
Results
General overview
We wanted to measure fundamental responses to grating components, both alone and when forming part of a plaid. We also wanted to measure intermodulation responses at the difference and sum of the fundamental frequencies used to modulate the contrast of each component forming a plaid. Clear component-based responses at ƒ1 (2.3 Hz) and ƒ2 (3.75 Hz) were observed at posterior occipital sites for gratings presented alone as well as when they were presented along with another grating component in the plaid conditions. Further, clear intermodulation responses were observed in all plaid conditions at ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz), but only in coherent plaid conditions at 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz). In both cases these were observed at the same sites as the component responses (Figure 4). 
Figure 4
 
Example Fourier amplitude spectra with SNR topographies. Top: Grating component A1 alone with fundamental SNR topography. Bottom: Coherent plaid A1A2 with ƒ1 + ƒ2 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 intermodulation SNR topographies. A component alone resulted in fundamental responses at the driving frequency as well as harmonic responses. Simultaneously presenting two components resulted in fundamental and harmonic responses for each component frequency. Additional nonlinearities were observed at the intermodulation frequencies. The reason that a few responses appear to straddle multiple bins (the ƒ2, ƒ2 − ƒ1, and ƒ1 + ƒ2 responses) was that the stimulus frequencies fell exactly between two bins in these cases; the FFT bins had a resolution of 0.1 Hz.
Figure 4
 
Example Fourier amplitude spectra with SNR topographies. Top: Grating component A1 alone with fundamental SNR topography. Bottom: Coherent plaid A1A2 with ƒ1 + ƒ2 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 intermodulation SNR topographies. A component alone resulted in fundamental responses at the driving frequency as well as harmonic responses. Simultaneously presenting two components resulted in fundamental and harmonic responses for each component frequency. Additional nonlinearities were observed at the intermodulation frequencies. The reason that a few responses appear to straddle multiple bins (the ƒ2, ƒ2 − ƒ1, and ƒ1 + ƒ2 responses) was that the stimulus frequencies fell exactly between two bins in these cases; the FFT bins had a resolution of 0.1 Hz.
Response to components
To test the well-documented effects of XOS (DeAngelis et al., 1994; Meese & Holmes, 2007; Petrov et al., 2005) we could compare directly the response to each frequency-tagged grating component in isolation and in the presence of a second grating. The SNR measures of these responses to each component—calculated by dividing the amplitude at the frequency of interest by the average amplitude of the surrounding 12 frequencies (with signal bins excluded)—can be seen in Figure 5. Substantial suppression was observed for components presented at 2.3 Hz in the presence of another grating component at 3.75 Hz. This was not the case for components presented at 3.75 Hz. 
Figure 5
 
Bar plots displaying fundamental frequency response SNRs to Component A1, Component A2, Component B1 and Component B2. As indicated by the stimulus insets, the leftmost bar of each plot represents the fundamental frequency SNR when that component was presented alone, the middle when it was part of a coherent plaid, and the right as part of a noncoherent plaid. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05 and ** at p < 0.01.
Figure 5
 
Bar plots displaying fundamental frequency response SNRs to Component A1, Component A2, Component B1 and Component B2. As indicated by the stimulus insets, the leftmost bar of each plot represents the fundamental frequency SNR when that component was presented alone, the middle when it was part of a coherent plaid, and the right as part of a noncoherent plaid. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05 and ** at p < 0.01.
All fundamental component responses, whether presented alone or in the presence of another grating, were significantly above background noise (p < 0.01 in all cases). For the 2.3 Hz response to grating A1 there was a significant main effect of stimulus pattern, F(2, 28) = 8.26, p = 0.002. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that component A1 SNRs were reduced for both the A1A2 plaid, t(28) = 2.92, p = 0.018, and the A1B2 plaid, t(28) = 3.91, p = 0.002, but there was no significant difference between the two plaid conditions, t(28) = 0.99, p = 0.587. A similar effect was observed for 2.3 Hz responses to B1, F(2, 28) = 6.64, p = 0.004, again driven by significant differences between the grating alone and each plaid condition (B1B2: t[28] = 3.12, p = 0.009; B1A2: t[28] = 3.12, p = 0.011). There was no significant difference between the response to coherent and noncoherent plaids, t(28) = − 0.06, p = 0.998. 
For the higher temporal frequency components, at 3.75 Hz the levels of suppression were less pronounced and did not result in significant main effects of stimulus type (A2: F[2, 28] = 0.02, p = 0.98; B2: F[2, 28] = 0.65, p = 0.528). This pattern of suppression may reflect an additional complex interaction between spatial and temporal frequency tuning of cross-orientation normalization processes (Cass & Alais, 2006; Meese & Holmes, 2007, 2010). 
In summary, although different patterns of suppression were observed between components, levels of XOS were similar for both coherent and noncoherent plaids for all conditions. This indicates that suppressive effects were not spatial-frequency tuned. 
Response to plaids
Intermodulation frequencies were used to assess responses to the conjunction of grating components. Responses at the difference frequency (ƒ2 − ƒ1: 1.45 Hz) were not prominent compared to background noise for all conditions. A main effect of pattern was found, F(6, 84) = 3.09, p = 0.009, though no significant differences were observed in post hoc pairwise comparisons between plaid conditions. At the harmonic of the intermodulation difference frequency (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1: 2.9 Hz), responses in all conditions were not prominent compared to background noise, and the main effect of stimulus pattern on SNRs was nonsignificant, F(6, 84) = 2.07, p = 0.066. 
At the sum intermodulation frequency (ƒ1 + ƒ2: 6.05 Hz) prominent responses were observed (Figure 6). One-sample t tests comparing each condition's SNR to background noise revealed significant differences for both coherent plaid and noncoherent plaid conditions, A1A2: t(14) = 5.07, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [1.62, 2.53]; B1B2: t(14) = 7.91, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [2.70, 3.97]; A1B2: t(14) = 4.45, p = 0.003, 95% CI = [1.66, 2.88]; B1A2: t(14) = 3.38, p < 0.023, 95% CI = [1.41, 2.84], as well as for component A2, t(14) = 3.24, p < 0.024, 95% CI = [1.13, 1.65]. The latter finding was unexpected as only one component (i.e., only one flickering stimulus) was presented in that condition; mathematically, an intermodulation response should not take place. This finding is therefore likely spurious. A significant main effect of stimulus pattern was found, F(6, 84) = 24.23, p < 0.001. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that response SNRs at 6.05 Hz were significantly larger in response to plaid B1B2 than for plaid A1A2, t(84) = −4.22, p < 0.001, and the mean 6.05 Hz response SNR to the noncoherent plaids (the mean of A1B2 and B1A2 responses; t[84] = −3.00, p = 0.009). 
Figure 6
 
Bar plots displaying ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz) and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz) frequency response SNRs. The dashed black line represents an SNR of 1 (i.e., background noise) and error bars represent standard error of the mean. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05, ** at p < 0.01, and *** at p < 0.001.
Figure 6
 
Bar plots displaying ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz) and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz) frequency response SNRs. The dashed black line represents an SNR of 1 (i.e., background noise) and error bars represent standard error of the mean. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05, ** at p < 0.01, and *** at p < 0.001.
At the harmonic of the intermodulation sum frequency (2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 12.1 Hz; see Figure 6) only responses to coherent plaids were significantly above background noise, A1A2: t(14) = 3.74, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [1.71, 3.60]; B1B2: t(14) = 4.00, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [1.48, 2.82]. There was a significant main effect of stimulus pattern on response SNRs, F(6, 84) = 11.61, p < 0.001. Significant differences were observed in post hoc pairwise comparisons between both coherent plaids and the mean noncoherent plaid response, A1A2: t(84) = 4.69, p < 0.001; B1B2: t(84) = 2.62, p = 0.030. 
In summary, responses at the difference intermodulation terms analyzed here were not significantly above background noise and did not systematically differ based on plaid coherence. The response at 6.05 Hz was significantly above background noise for all plaid conditions, and was greater for coherent plaid B1B2 than any other condition. That all plaid responses were significantly above background noise and that suppression for coherent and noncoherent plaid responses was similar, may indicate that the 6.05 Hz response primarily reflected XOS. A more convincing selectivity was observed at 12.1 Hz; it was larger for coherent than noncoherent plaids, and was significantly above background noise for coherent plaids but not for noncoherent plaids, suggesting that it was spatial-frequency tuned. 
Modeling
Simulations of several candidate models, including the conjunction detection model outlined in the Methods section, were generated. The candidate models with no conjunction detection were each composed of a channel bank of four V1 neurons (like Layer 1 of the conjunction model). In the interest of understanding how different component-based nonlinearities contribute to the summed nonlinearities observed in EEG data, input signals underwent only contrast saturation in one model, and both contrast saturation and XOS in another. The output of these simulations is shown in the top rows of Figures 7b and 8b. In both figures, the model output is being compared to the EEG data we collected for coherent plaid A1A2. The EEG data is shown in section (a) of both figures, and the various model outputs are shown in section (b). The same noise as described earlier was injected into the output of these. Two versions of each were run; one where no NIR was applied to the channel output (Figure 7b) and another where the NIRs that were outlined earlier were applied (Figure 8b). It should be noted that the application of the second NIR (whether bandpass or high pass) was applied to Layer 2 in the conjunction detection model only (shown in the bottom row of Figures 7b and 8b). 
Figure 7
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data without an NIR being applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection model.
Figure 7
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data without an NIR being applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection model.
Figure 8
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data with NIRs applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection models.
Figure 8
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data with NIRs applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection models.
In the first candidate model, each channel had a stage of contrast saturation upon contrast input (Figure 7b top left; Figure 8b top left). Fundamental responses and their harmonics were produced. No intermodulation responses were produced because the operation performed by each channel was independent of the other channels; the different temporal signals could not combine. This is dissimilar to the observed EEG data in sections (a) of both figures, and clearly not the case for much of the visual system. In V1 substantial XOS is usually observed in response to superimposed grating stimuli (e.g., Bonds, 1989; Brouwer & Heeger, 2011; Burr & Morrone, 1987). 
The second candidate model performed contrast saturation and received input from a normalization pool (outlined in Methods section)—that is, a model of contrast gain to account for contrast saturation and XOS (Figure 7b top right; Figure 8b top right). When no NIR was applied, suppression was observed at both the fundamental and harmonic component-based SNRs. With the application of the NIR to the output of each channel, suppression was observed at the fundamental response frequencies, but the harmonic SNRs showed a slight increase. In both cases, a substantial intermodulation response SNR was observed at 6.05 Hz (ƒ1 + ƒ2) and a slight intermodulation response was observed at 12.1 Hz (2ƒ1 + 2ƒ1). This supports the finding that XOS drives substantial intermodulation responses (e.g., Baker et al., 2011; Candy et al., 2001). However, the output produced by these models is still dissimilar to the raw data in showing relatively weak harmonic component responses (and essentially no harmonic intermodulation response). 
When no NIR was applied, the additional nonlinearity of the conjunction detector raised the SNR at the fundamental, harmonic, and intermodulation terms compared to the contrast gain (contrast saturation + XOS) only model. In contrast, the addition of the bandpass NIR at Layer 1 and the high-pass NIR at Layer 2 resulted in larger SNRs only at the harmonic responses (4.6 Hz: 2ƒ1, 7.5 Hz: 2ƒ2, and 12.1 Hz: 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2). Applying the bandpass NIR to both Layers 1 and 2 of the conjunction model decreased fundamental component responses, the 4.6 Hz response and the 12.1 Hz response, but notably increased the 6.05 Hz response. This latter model was the most dissimilar of the conjunction detector models to the observed EEG data. The model with no NIR and the bandpass + high-pass model both resulted in larger 12.1 Hz SNRs, the frequency at which a significant difference between coherent and noncoherent plaids was found in the EEG data. They also display several additional frequency combination responses that are present within the EEG data spectra, such as the response at 9.8 Hz (ƒ1 + 2ƒ2). Both clearly capture some of the subtleties involved in the signal combinations taking place in the EEG data, though the larger 12.1 Hz response produced by the bandpass + high-pass model is closer to the EEG findings. 
In summary, we generated several simple fixed models to account for different kinds of signal summation in response to frequency-tagged flickering stimuli. Rather than comparing models using measures of fit quality and (somewhat arbitrary) penalties for numbers of parameters, we have compared models in terms of whether they produced responses at the expected frequencies. The only model variants capable of producing the full range of responses observed in the data were the models including a second layer nonlinear step; the model with only XOS (normalization) does not show the observed intermodulation responses. The use of a bandpass NIR at Layer 1 and a higher pass NIR at Layer 2 of the conjunction model appears to slightly better account for our data than without the application of any NIR, and much better than when a bandpass NIR is applied to both Layers 1 and 2 of the model. This presumably indicates different temporal integration windows at different stages of the visual hierarchy. 
Discussion
We have measured neural responses to sinusoidal grating patterns presented alone and combined as coherent and noncoherent plaids to assess the nonlinear combination of neural responses to the gratings. To do this we measured EEG responses at intermodulation frequencies, which have previously been shown to indicate a nonlinearity at or after the point of summation (Regan, 1983; Regan & Heron, 1969; Regan & Regan, 1988; Spekreijse & Oosting, 1970; Spekreijse & Reits, 1982; Zemon & Ratliff, 1984). Our main finding was that for compound stimuli (plaids) we found reliable responses at 12.1 Hz (2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2), but only when the combination formed a coherent plaid. 
Although this intermodulation response indicates a nonlinear combination of signals, it does not tell us the nature of that nonlinearity. In particular it might simply reflect lateral suppressive effects such as XOS (Baker et al., 2011; Candy et al., 2001). The frequency-tagging technique we have used allows us to investigate that hypothesis directly by examining the effect of each component (e.g., presented at 3.75 Hz) on the response to the other (at 2.3 Hz). We did indeed observe substantial suppression; for example, when a component (either A1 or B1) was presented alone at 2.3 Hz, a greater 2.3 Hz response was observed than when the component was presented in combination with an orthogonal grating. 
Critically, however, the 12.1 Hz response was very much dependent on the spatial frequencies being matched in the two components. Conversely, and in keeping with previous findings that XOS is largely untuned for spatial frequency (DeAngelis et al., 1994; Petrov et al., 2005), the reductions in component responses that we measured occurred equally for any combination of spatial frequencies. It seems unlikely that the observed intermodulation responses resulted purely from XOS, given that one is tuned for spatial frequency and the other is not. 
The perception of moving plaids has been shown to depend on matched spatial frequencies in several ways. When spatial frequencies differ in the two gratings being combined, observers perceive a pair of semitransparent gratings sliding past each other (Adelson & Movshon, 1982) whereas a plaid with matched spatial frequency components appears as a single coherent checkerboard pattern with a single direction of motion. Similarly, selective adaptation to static plaids decreases when components are unmatched (Hancock et al., 2010) and the pop-out effect in visual search disappears when plaid targets have unmatched components (Nam et al., 2009). A mechanism selective for coherent plaids appears to explain better the nonlinear intermodulation responses we have measured. In support of this, we found that the addition of “conjunction detector” channels beyond contrast gain operations resulted in model output more like the EEG data. A logical AND operation in combination with XOS cannot be ruled out as a candidate mechanism for driving plaid-selective responses at intermodulation frequencies. 
Studies of fMRI (McDonald, Mannion, & Clifford, 2012) and positron emission topography (PET) (Wenderoth, Watson, Egan, Tochon-Danguy, & O'Keefe, 1999) have shown similar responses to plaid and grating stimuli of equivalent contrast. McDonald et al. (2012) used fMRI to study the summation of signals to gratings and plaids. However, this method measures a mixed signal; it cannot distinguish changes in the responses of a set of neurons from changes in the number of neurons responding. For instance, a greater response in V1 could be caused either by reduced suppression between neurons or by recruitment of an additional mechanism. The inherently superior temporal resolution of EEG, combined with the frequency-tagging technique, allows us to separate responses to different components within the stimulus. 
Previous measurements using the frequency tagging technique with combinations of oriented gratings have shown intermodulation responses (Baker et al., 2011; Candy et al., 2001). These were attributed to suppression between channels; coherent and noncoherent plaids were not compared in terms of spatial frequency matching. These studies also used counterphase flicker, whereas we modulated between low and high contrast intensities and measured fundamental responses at the driving frequencies. The harmonic responses at 2ƒ1 (4.6 Hz) and 2ƒ2 (7.5 Hz)—not shown here—displayed the same effects as the fundamental responses in terms of component suppression; all components displayed the same amount of harmonic component suppression irrespective of plaid coherence. 
The intermodulation responses we measured were predominantly observed at the sum intermodulation frequencies, and not at the difference. This has also been the case for several other intermodulation studies (Aissani et al., 2011; Alp et al., 2016; Appelbaum et al., 2008; Gundlach & Müller, 2013) but the converse was true for Boremanse et al. (2013). One interpretation for differences between the intermodulation terms suggested by Boremanse et al. (2013) is that responses at both frequencies reflect parallel nonlinearities but the sum intermodulation response output may be a temporally band- or high-pass nonlinearity and signal early local spatial interactions, whereas the difference intermodulation response may be generated by a temporally low-pass nonlinearity and generated by signal integration to higher level (global) stimuli, such as their face-part stimuli, which require longer to process (Alonso-Prieto, Van Belle, Liu-Shuang, Norcia, & Rossion, 2013). Here we found that the application of a bandpass temporal filter at Layer 1 and a higher frequency filter at Layer 2 of our model resulted in better model output than by applying a bandpass filter at both layers. Rapid local combinations would certainly fit in with our EEG results as we used simple sinusoidal gratings that presumably were being combined across many receptive fields to encode another pattern (the plaid). Further, sum intermodulation responses were strongest around Oz, placed approximately over the occipital pole, consistent with activity relatively early in visual cortex. 
Alp et al. (2016) suggested that the temporal resonance properties of different neural mechanisms may influence the varied response at the difference and sum intermodulation frequencies. These resonances may depend on specific synaptic connections to and from the mechanisms, feedback connectivity and the relative complexity of the receptive field within the visual hierarchy (e.g., sensitive to compound plaids or sensitive to faces). The effects of such differences in temporal integration have not been applied quantitatively in a computational model (e.g., to explain differential responses at sum- and difference-intermodulation terms). Here we used a simple approach to model the temporal properties of mechanisms and found that using different neural impulse response functions (temporal filters) at early and late layers was sufficient to explain a wide range of features in the data. In the case studied here (plaid combinations) it appeared that responses at 6.05 Hz (ƒ1 + ƒ2) in the present data primarily represented XOS mechanisms, while responses at 12.1 Hz (2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2) reflected plaid-selective mechanisms. 
The responses to gratings and plaids appear similar in terms of their topography. Although they may result from different neurons, we would expect these to be anatomically proximal. For example, if the response to plaids originated in V2, it would be hard to distinguish from the V1 response to gratings using EEG. Furthermore, although they are reliable, the intermodulation responses do not have large amplitudes, which compounds the difficulty in localizing them. 
Conclusion
In summary, we have shown a nonlinear response to a compound of gratings (plaid) that does not arise purely from contrast normalization between spatial frequency channels. The data are in keeping with a mechanism for detecting conjunctions of visual features, as might result from a logical AND operation. The frequency-tagging technique provides a useful tool to investigate AND gates in a wide variety of neural mechanisms. 
Acknowledgments
DC was funded by a studentship from the EPSRC, UK. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Jonathan W. Peirce. 
Address: Nottingham Visual Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University Park Campus, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, Nottinghamshire, UK. 
References
Adelson, E. H., & Movshon, J. A. (1982). Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns. Nature, 300 (5892), 523–525. Retrieved from http://www.cns.nyu.edu/∼msl/courses/2223/Readings/Adelson-Nature1982.pdf
Aissani, C., Cottereau, B. R., Dumas, G., Paradis, A.-L., & Lorenceau, J. (2011). Magnetoencephalographic signatures of visual form and motion binding. Brain Research, 1408, 27–40, doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.051.
Alonso-Prieto, E., Van Belle, G., Liu-Shuang, J., Norcia, A. M., & Rossion, B. (2013). The 6Hz fundamental stimulation frequency rate for individual face discrimination in the right occipito-temporal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 51 (13), 2863–2875, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.018.
Alp, N., Kogo, N., Van Belle, G., Wagemans, J., & Rossion, B. (2016). Frequency tagging yields an objective neural signature of Gestalt formation. Brain and Cognition, 104, 15–24, doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2016.01.008.
Appelbaum, L. G., & Norcia, A. M. (2009). Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing. Journal of Vision, 9 (11): 18, 1–12, doi:10.1167/9.11.18. [PubMed] [Article]
Appelbaum, L. G., Wade, A. R., Pettet, M. W., Vildavski, V. Y., & Norcia, A. M. (2008). Figure–ground interaction in the human visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8 (9): 8, 1–19, doi:10.1167/8.9.8. [PubMed] [Article]
Baker, T. J., Norcia, A. M., & RowanCandy, T. (2011). Orientation tuning in the visual cortex of 3-month-old human infants. Vision Research, 51 (5), 470–478, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.01.003.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68 (3), 255–278, doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.
Bonds, A. B. (1989). Role of inhibition in the specification of orientation selectivity of cells in the cat striate cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 2 (1), 41–55, doi:10.1017/S0952523800004314.
Boremanse, A., Norcia, A. M., & Rossion, B. (2013). An objective signature for visual binding of face parts in the human brain. Journal of Vision, 13 (11): 6, 1–18, doi:10.1167/13.11.6. [PubMed] [Article]
Brouwer, G. J., & Heeger, D. J. (2011). Cross-orientation suppression in human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106 (5), 2108–2119, doi:10.1152/jn.00540.2011.
Burr, D. C., & Morrone, M. C. (1987). Inhibitory interactions in the human vision system revealed in pattern-evoked potentials. The Journal of Physiology, 389 (1), 1–21, doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016643.
Candy, T. R., Skoczenski, A. M., & Norcia, A. M. (2001). Normalization models applied to orientation masking in the human infant. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21 (12), 4530–4541. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/content/21/12/4530
Carandini, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2012). Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13 (1), 51–62, doi:10.1038/nrn3136.
Cass, J., & Alais, D. (2006). Evidence for two interacting temporal channels in human visual processing. Vision Research, 46 (18), 2859–2868, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.02.015.
DeAngelis, G. C., Freeman, R. D., & Ohzawa, I. (1994). Length and width tuning of neurons in the cat's primary visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 71 (1), 347–374. Retrieved from http://jn.physiology.org/content/71/1/347.short
Gheorghiu, E., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2009). Multiplication in curvature processing. Journal of Vision, 9 (2): 23, 1–17, doi:10.1167/9.2.23. [PubMed] [Article]
Gundlach, C., & Müller, M. M. (2013). Perception of illusory contours forms intermodulation responses of steady state visual evoked potentials as a neural signature of spatial integration. Biological Psychology, 94 (1), 55–60, doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.014.
Hancock, S., McGovern, D. P., & Peirce, J. W. (2010). Ameliorating the combinatorial explosion with spatial frequency-matched combinations of V1 outputs. Journal of Vision, 10 (8): 7, 1–14, doi:10.1167/10.8.7. [PubMed] [Article]
Hancock, S., & Peirce, J. W. (2008). Selective mechanisms for simple contours revealed by compound adaptation. Journal of Vision, 8 (7): 11, 1–10, doi:10.1167/10.8.7. [PubMed] [Article]
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6 (2), 65–70. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/4615733
Huk, A. C., Ress, D., & Heeger, D. J. (2001). Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron, 6 (2), 65–70.
Kanizsa, G. (1979). Organization in vision: Essays on gestalt perception. New York: Praeger. Retrieved from https://books.google.com/books?id=iogQAQAAIAAJ&pgis=1
McDonald, J. S., Mannion, D. J., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2012). Gain control in the response of human visual cortex to plaids. Journal of Neurophysiology, 107 (9), 2570–2580, doi:10.1152/jn.00616.2011.
McGovern, D. P., & Peirce, J. W. (2007). The effect of contrast on adaptation to compound patterns. Journal of Vision, 7 (9): 265, doi:10.1167/7.9.265. [Abstract]
Meese, T. S., & Freeman, T. C. (1995). Edge computation in human vision: Anisotropy in the combining of oriented filters. Perception, 24 (6), 603–622, doi:10.1068/p240603.
Meese, T. S., & Holmes, D. J. (2007). Spatial and temporal dependencies of cross-orientation suppression in human vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274 (1606), 127–136, doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3697.
Meese, T. S., & Holmes, D. J. (2010). Orientation masking and cross-orientation suppression (XOS): Implications for estimates of filter bandwidth. Journal of Vision, 10 (12): 9, 1–20, doi:10.1167/10.12.9. [PubMed] [Article]
Naka, K. I., & Rushton, W. A. H. (1966). S-potentials from luminosity units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae). The Journal of Physiology, 185 (3), 587–599, doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008003.
Nam, J.-H., Solomon, J. A., Morgan, M. J., Wright, C. E., & Chubb, C. (2009). Coherent plaids are preattentively more than the sum of their parts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71 (7), 1469–1477, doi:10.3758/APP.71.7.1469.
Norcia, A. M., Appelbaum, L. G., Ales, J. M., Cottereau, B. R., & Rossion, G. (2015). The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research: A review. Journal of Vision, 15 (6): 4, 1–46, doi:10.1167/15.6.4. [PubMed] [Article]
Peirce, J. W. (2007a). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162 (1–2), 8–13, doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017.
Peirce, J. W. (2007b). The potential importance of saturating and supersaturating contrast response functions in visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 7 (6): 13, 1–10, doi:10.1167/7.6.13. [PubMed] [Article]
Peirce, J. W. (2011). Nonlinear summation really can be used to perform AND operations: Reply to May and Zhaoping. Journal of Vision, 11 (9): 18, 1–3, doi:10.1167/11.9.18. [PubMed] [Article]
Peirce, J. W. (2015). Understanding mid-level representations in visual processing. Journal of Vision, 15 (7): 5, 1–9, doi:10.1167/15.7.5. [PubMed] [Article]
Peirce, J. W., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Selective mechanisms for complex visual patterns revealed by adaptation. Neuroscience, 141 (1), 15–18, doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.039.
Petrov, Y., Carandini, M., & McKee, S. P. (2005). Two distinct mechanisms of suppression in human vision. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25 (38), 8704–8707, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2871-05.2005.
Regan, D. (1983). Spatial frequency mechanisms in human vision investigated by evoked potential recording. Vision Research, 23 (12), 1401–1407, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(83)90151-7.
Regan, D., & Heron, J. R. (1969). Clinical investigation of lesions of the visual pathway: A new objective technique. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 32 (5), 479–483. Retrieved from http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=496563&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype= abstract
Regan, M. P., & Regan, D. (1988). A frequency domain technique for characterizing nonlinearities in biological systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 133 (3), 293–317, doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(88)80323-0.
Robinson, A., & MacLeod, D. (2011). The McCollough effect with plaids and gratings: Evidence for a plaid-selective visual mechanism. Journal of Vision, 11 (1): 26, 1–9, doi:10.1167/11.1.26. [PubMed] [Article]
Russell, L. V. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. 69 (1), 1–33, doi:10.18637/jss.v069.i01.
Schwartz, O., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2001). Natural signal statistics and sensory gain control. Nature Neuroscience, 4 (8), 819–825, doi:10.1038/90526.
Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., Højsgaard, S., Fox, J.,… Mertens, U. (2016). afex: Analysis of factorial experiments. Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN). Retrieved from https://github.com/singmann/afex
Spekreijse, H., & Oosting, H. (1970). Linearizing: A method for analysing and synthesizing nonlinear systems. Kybernetik, 7 (1), 22–31, doi:10.1007/BF00270331.
Spekreijse, H., & Reits, D. (1982). Sequential analysis of the visual evoked potentials system in man; nonlinear analysis of a sandwich system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 338 (1), 72–97.
Tsai, J. J., Wade, A. R., & Norcia, A. M. (2012). Dynamics of normalization underlying masking in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32 (8), 2783–2789, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4485-11.2012.
Wenderoth, P., Watson, J. D., Egan, G. F., Tochon-Danguy, H. J., & O'Keefe, G. J. (1999). Second order components of moving plaids activate extrastriate cortex: A positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage, 9 (2), 227–234, doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0398.
Zemon, V., & Ratliff, F. (1984). Intermodulation components of the visual evoked potential: Responses to lateral and superimposed stimuli. Biological Cybernetics, 50 (6), 401–408, doi:10.1007/BF00335197.
Figure 1
 
Grating/plaid examples. Left: Grating component alone examples. Middle: Coherent plaid examples. Note that these always look like checkerboards; one cannot “choose” to perceive them as a pair of gratings. Right: Noncoherent plaid examples in which the pattern can be perceived as a pair of gratings without any automatic fusing as a single checkerboard pattern. The top row represents configurations for the 1 cpd components, and the bottom row the 3 cpd components, although these are only for illustrative purposes rather than being drawn to scale.
Figure 1
 
Grating/plaid examples. Left: Grating component alone examples. Middle: Coherent plaid examples. Note that these always look like checkerboards; one cannot “choose” to perceive them as a pair of gratings. Right: Noncoherent plaid examples in which the pattern can be perceived as a pair of gratings without any automatic fusing as a single checkerboard pattern. The top row represents configurations for the 1 cpd components, and the bottom row the 3 cpd components, although these are only for illustrative purposes rather than being drawn to scale.
Figure 2
 
Model schematic displaying how contrast input was transformed within each layer. Though the Layer 1 channels were untuned in this study, the Gabor patches differing in orientation and spatial frequency are visual representations used to indicate how the channels, within the framework that we propose, might be tuned. These are followed by a static sigmoidal nonlinearity and then a temporal filter (if appropriate to the simulation). The normalization pool exerted its influence (for XOS) as part of the static nonlinearity. A similar configuration (without normalization) was used to transform input into Layer 2 channels.
Figure 2
 
Model schematic displaying how contrast input was transformed within each layer. Though the Layer 1 channels were untuned in this study, the Gabor patches differing in orientation and spatial frequency are visual representations used to indicate how the channels, within the framework that we propose, might be tuned. These are followed by a static sigmoidal nonlinearity and then a temporal filter (if appropriate to the simulation). The normalization pool exerted its influence (for XOS) as part of the static nonlinearity. A similar configuration (without normalization) was used to transform input into Layer 2 channels.
Figure 3
 
Temporal and Fourier responses of the bandpass and higher pass temporal filters. The left figures represent temporal responses and the right Fourier responses. Red indicates responses of the bandpass filter and blue the higher pass filter.
Figure 3
 
Temporal and Fourier responses of the bandpass and higher pass temporal filters. The left figures represent temporal responses and the right Fourier responses. Red indicates responses of the bandpass filter and blue the higher pass filter.
Figure 4
 
Example Fourier amplitude spectra with SNR topographies. Top: Grating component A1 alone with fundamental SNR topography. Bottom: Coherent plaid A1A2 with ƒ1 + ƒ2 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 intermodulation SNR topographies. A component alone resulted in fundamental responses at the driving frequency as well as harmonic responses. Simultaneously presenting two components resulted in fundamental and harmonic responses for each component frequency. Additional nonlinearities were observed at the intermodulation frequencies. The reason that a few responses appear to straddle multiple bins (the ƒ2, ƒ2 − ƒ1, and ƒ1 + ƒ2 responses) was that the stimulus frequencies fell exactly between two bins in these cases; the FFT bins had a resolution of 0.1 Hz.
Figure 4
 
Example Fourier amplitude spectra with SNR topographies. Top: Grating component A1 alone with fundamental SNR topography. Bottom: Coherent plaid A1A2 with ƒ1 + ƒ2 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 intermodulation SNR topographies. A component alone resulted in fundamental responses at the driving frequency as well as harmonic responses. Simultaneously presenting two components resulted in fundamental and harmonic responses for each component frequency. Additional nonlinearities were observed at the intermodulation frequencies. The reason that a few responses appear to straddle multiple bins (the ƒ2, ƒ2 − ƒ1, and ƒ1 + ƒ2 responses) was that the stimulus frequencies fell exactly between two bins in these cases; the FFT bins had a resolution of 0.1 Hz.
Figure 5
 
Bar plots displaying fundamental frequency response SNRs to Component A1, Component A2, Component B1 and Component B2. As indicated by the stimulus insets, the leftmost bar of each plot represents the fundamental frequency SNR when that component was presented alone, the middle when it was part of a coherent plaid, and the right as part of a noncoherent plaid. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05 and ** at p < 0.01.
Figure 5
 
Bar plots displaying fundamental frequency response SNRs to Component A1, Component A2, Component B1 and Component B2. As indicated by the stimulus insets, the leftmost bar of each plot represents the fundamental frequency SNR when that component was presented alone, the middle when it was part of a coherent plaid, and the right as part of a noncoherent plaid. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05 and ** at p < 0.01.
Figure 6
 
Bar plots displaying ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz) and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz) frequency response SNRs. The dashed black line represents an SNR of 1 (i.e., background noise) and error bars represent standard error of the mean. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05, ** at p < 0.01, and *** at p < 0.001.
Figure 6
 
Bar plots displaying ƒ1 + ƒ2 (6.05 Hz) and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2 (12.1 Hz) frequency response SNRs. The dashed black line represents an SNR of 1 (i.e., background noise) and error bars represent standard error of the mean. * represents a significant difference at p < 0.05, ** at p < 0.01, and *** at p < 0.001.
Figure 7
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data without an NIR being applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection model.
Figure 7
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data without an NIR being applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection model.
Figure 8
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data with NIRs applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection models.
Figure 8
 
SNR Spectra for (a) EEG data and (b) model data with NIRs applied. Highlighted in dark blue are fundamental component-based SNRs (ƒ1 and ƒ2: 2.3 Hz and 3.75 Hz), light blue the component harmonic SNRs (2ƒ1 and 2ƒ2: 4.6 Hz and 7.5 Hz), red the intermodulation responses (ƒ2 − ƒ1 and ƒ1 + ƒ2: 1.45 Hz and 6.05 Hz), and magenta the intermodulation harmonic responses (2ƒ2 − 2ƒ1 and 2ƒ1 + 2ƒ2: 2.9 Hz and 12.1 Hz). The contrast saturation model was the simplest, followed by the inclusion of XOS (normalization pool) and then the conjunction detection models.
Supplement 1
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×