Regardless of the details of the neural mechanism, the brain clearly solves both egocentric and allocentric tasks. Human behavioral studies have shown that in the absence of allocentric cues, saccades and reaches toward remembered targets are reasonably accurate but show various posture and gaze-dependent errors (Blohm & Crawford,
2007; Bock,
1986; Crawford et al.,
2011; Henriques, Klier, Smith, Lowy, & Crawford,
1998). Additionally, humans are able to reach toward locations defined relative to a mobile visual landmark with varying degrees of accuracy (Byrne, Cappadocia, & Crawford,
2010; Chen et al.,
2014). When viewing a natural environment, both egocentric and allocentric information are normally available, and it is thought that the brain combines both sources of information (Battaglia-Mayer, Caminiti, Lacquaniti, & Zago,
2003; Diedrichsen, Werner, Schmidt, & Trommershauser,
2004; Sheth & Shimojo,
2004). This has primarily been studied in reach studies. When allocentric information agrees with egocentric cues, it tends to improve the accuracy and precision of movements toward a remembered target location (Krigolson & Heath,
2004; Krigolson, Clark, Heath, & Binsted,
2007; Obhi & Goodale,
2005; Thaler & Goodale,
2011). However, when these cues conflict they are weighted based on their relative reliabilities (Byrne & Crawford,
2010; Fiehler et al.,
2014; Thompson & Henriques,
2010). Furthermore, wide range of variables have been shown to influence these interactions, such as age (Hanisch et al.,
2001; Lemay et al.,
2004), memory delay (Carrozzo et al.,
2002; Chen et al.,
2011; Glover & Dixon,
2004; Hay & Redon,
2006; Obhi & Goodale,
2005), context (Neely, Tessmer, Binsted, & Heath,
2008), task demands (Bridgeman et al.,
1997), size of the allocentric landmark (Inoue et al.,
2015; Uchimura & Kitazawa,
2013), location of the landmark relative to reach direction (de Grave, Brenner, & Smeets,
2004), and perceived stability of the landmark (Byrne & Crawford,
2010; Byrne et al.,
2010).