Saccadic eye movements provide a fundamental possibility to explore our visual environment and offer the opportunity to investigate basic mechanisms of sensorimotor control (Krauzlis,
2005). Indeed, age-related changes in saccadic control have been explored by numerous studies. Results consistently support a pronounced increase of latencies with age but stability of saccade dynamics (Abel & Douglas,
2007; Munoz, Broughton, Goldring, & Armstrong,
1998; Peltsch, Hemraj, Garcia, & Munoz,
2011). A further robust finding is an age-related increase of directional error rates in saccade tasks that require inhibitory control processes (e.g., in the antisaccade task; Gottlob, Fillmore, & Abroms,
2007; Olk & Jin,
2011; Peltsch et al.,
2011; Sweeney, Rosano, Berman, & Luna,
2001). In particular, saccade accuracy seems to be maintained stable across the adult life span, even up to an age of 80 years (Munoz et al.,
1998). This suggests preserved capacities to compensate for possible age-related changes of ocular muscles or neuronal circuitries that might challenge saccadic accuracy. Age-related changes in plasticity of saccadic control have remained poorly understood. Evidence of robust saccadic adaptation in the developmental course comes from several studies in children and adolescents (Alahyane et al.,
2016; Doré-Mazars, Vergilino-Perez, Lemoine, & Bucci,
2011; Lemoine-Lardennois et al.,
2016; Salman et al.,
2006). Results indicate early maturation of adaptive capacities, showing, for example, reactive amplitude changes already in toddlers. However, only a single study so far has been dedicated to aging of saccadic adaptation and has provided evidence of similar capacities in different adult age groups (Bock, Ilieva, & Grigorova,
2014).