Let us assume that the spike count function,
R(
θ), is separable, and can be written as a multiplication of two functions:
g(
τ) that accounts for the spike count for a given window
τ, and
f(
θ) that modulates the spike count for different stimulus intensities. Further, we can say that for presentation times over approximately 75 ms, we can assume that the firing rate is falling [See Heller et al. (
1995) or figure 8 in Albrecht et al. (
2002) for evidence.] This means that
g(
τ) is less than linear, and can be approximated with the following equation:
\begin{equation}\tag{A2}g\left( \tau \right) = {\tau ^\eta }\qquad {\rm{for}}\;0 \lt \eta \lt 1\end{equation}
With these assumptions, we can begin to show that a variable firing rate will not effect the conclusion in the main paper. Inserting our separable
R(
θ) function into
Equation 1 from the main text, we arrive at
\begin{equation}\tag{A3}\sigma \left( \tau \right) = {{{\sigma _R}} \over {g(\tau )f^{\prime} (\theta )}}\end{equation}
Following the same process we did in the previous section where we created a ratio between
σθ(
τ1) and
σθ(
τsat), we arrive at
\begin{equation}\tag{A4}{{{\sigma _\theta }\left( {{\tau _1}} \right)} \over {{\sigma _\theta }\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right)}} = {{{\sigma _R}\left( {{\tau _1}} \right)} \over {g\left( {{\tau _1}} \right){f^{\prime} }(\theta )}}/{{{\sigma _R}\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right)} \over {g\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right){f^{\prime} }(\theta )}}\end{equation}
Rearranging and combining with the power law in assumption 3 from the main paper,
\begin{equation}\tag{A5}{{{\sigma _\theta }\left( {{\tau _1}} \right)} \over {{\sigma _\theta }\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right)}} = {{JN{D_\tau }} \over {JN{D_{{\rm{sat}}}}}} = {{g\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right)} \over {g\left( {{\tau _1}} \right)}}\cdot{{\beta \cdot g{{\left( {{\tau _1}} \right)}^\rho }f{{(\theta )}^\rho }} \over {\beta \cdot g{{\left( {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}} \right)}^\rho }f{{(\theta )}^\rho }}}\end{equation}
\begin{equation}\tag{A6}{{JN{D_\tau }} \over {JN{D_{{\rm{sat}}}}}} = {\left( {{{g({\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}})} \over {g({\tau _1})}}} \right)^{1 - \rho }}\end{equation}
Plugging
Equation A2 in to
Equation A6,
\begin{equation}\tag{A7}{{JN{D_\tau }} \over {JN{D_{{\rm{sat}}}}}} = \left( {{{{\tau _1}} \over {{\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}}}} \right){^{\eta \cdot\left( {\rho - 1} \right)}}\end{equation}
Taking the logarithm of both sides allows us to compare to
Equation 4 from the main paper,
\begin{equation}\tag{A8}LJR = \log \left( {{{JN{D_\tau }} \over {JN{D_{{\rm{sat}}}}}}} \right) = \eta \cdot\left( {\rho - 1} \right)\cdot\log \left( {{\tau _1}/{\tau _{sat}}} \right)\qquad {\rm{for}}\;\tau \le {\tau _{{\rm{sat}}}}\end{equation}
Just like with
Equation 4, if we plot
LJR against log(
τ1), the slope (
m) of this line will be equivalent to the coefficient of the above equation:
\begin{equation}\tag{A9}m = \eta \cdot(\rho - 1)\end{equation}
Experimentally, we have found
m = −1.11 ± 0.09. From
Equation A9,
η would have to equal 2.64 for our original hypothesis of
ρ = 0.58 to be true. As stated when we introduced the
η variable, a biologically reasonable approximation for
η would be 0 <
η < 1. This implies that, even assuming a dynamically varying firing rate, we would still estimate
ρ < 0. Thus, for physiologically plausible firing rate dynamics, the behavioral statistics are inconsistent with the premise of spike count variability as the source of behavioral noise.