Open Access
Article  |   August 2017
Speed–size illusion correlates with retinal-level motion statistics
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision August 2017, Vol.17, 1. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/17.9.1
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Zixin Yong, Po-Jang Hsieh; Speed–size illusion correlates with retinal-level motion statistics. Journal of Vision 2017;17(9):1. https://doi.org/10.1167/17.9.1.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

It is a common perceptual experience that smaller objects appear to move faster than larger ones when their physical speeds are the same in either the laboratory or daily life. In this study, we show that the speed–size illusion is correlated with retinal image speed distribution bias. The illusion was quantified with a two-alternative, forced choice speed comparison paradigm, and retinal image speed distributions for different image sizes were obtained by simulation. Simulation results show that smaller retinal images tend to have slower projected speed, and the retinal image speed distribution bias correlates with the strength of the speed–size illusion. Furthermore, exposure to a training movie containing unnatural motion statistics tended to modulate the illusion in a way that was consistent with the speed distribution bias. We discuss how the data could be explained by empirical ranking theory, Bayesian theory, and motion adaptation.

Introduction
The human visual system exploits information carried by the pervasive electromagnetic waves in the environment to guide behaviors. Conversely, the environment shapes and constrains the visual system through natural selection (Banks, Sprague, Schmoll, Parnell, & Love, 2015; Land & Nilsson, 2012). The intricate relationship between the environment and visual perception suggests tight links exist between the regularities of the visual environment and perception (Geisler, 2008). Studies have revealed such correspondence by analyzing statistical properties of natural scenes and comparing them to psychophysical or neurophysiological measurements (Girshick, Landy, & Simoncelli, 2011; Laughlin, 1981; Liu, Bovik, & Cormack, 2008; Yang & Purves, 2003). This approach alludes to possible strategies our visual system employs to obtain useful information (Geisler, 2008). 
Visual motion perception does not always agree with physical measurements (Blakemore & Snowden, 1999; Webster, 2015). Misjudgments about speed are frequently observed in the psychophysics laboratory as well as daily situations (Leibowitz, 1985). Perceptual deviations that disagree with the immediate physical measurements of speed, however, may be explained by natural environmental statistics (Weiss, Simoncelli, & Adelson, 2002). Here we show that speed–size illusion, with which smaller objects appear to move faster than larger ones with identical physical speed, is correlated with the motion statistics at the retinal level. 
It is known that the size of an object affects estimation of its speed (Mckee & Smallman, 1998). Experiments using standard monitor or virtual reality presentation showed that smaller objects and vehicles appeared to move faster (Barton, 2006; Clark, Perrone, & Isler, 2013; Distler, Gegenfurtner, Van Veen, & Hawken, 2000). However, these experiments often involved factors other than size, such as change in size (Clark et al., 2013), distance perception (Barton, 2006; Distler et al., 2000), and familiarity of objects (Distler et al., 2000). Here we quantified the speed–size illusion with a two-alternative, forced choice (2AFC) speed comparison paradigm, in which white disks translated on a black background in the fronto-parallel plane. Because there was no distance cue of any sort being presented and the change in object size was undiscernible, size was the only primary factor other than physical speed that might contribute to speed perception. Motion statistics were obtained by simulating a virtual 3-D environment in which moving objects projected onto a 2-D retina-like image plane. Smaller retinal images were shown to have right-skewed speed distributions that fell off more quickly. Last, we demonstrated that recent visual experiences embedded with unnatural speed–size statistics tended to modulate the illusion in a manner that was consistent with the retinal image speed distribution bias. 
Methods
Quantifying the speed-size illusion (Experiment 1)
Eighteen (nine female, 19–31 years old) and 16 (eight female, 21–25 years old) naive participants with normal or corrected-to-normal vision took part in Experiment 1a and b, respectively. Informed consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki, and the participants were reimbursed S$10 for the 1-hr experiment. Stimuli were rendered on a Dell P170Sb monitor (75 Hz, 1280 × 1024 pixels, 23.8° × 19.2° view angle) with Psychtoolbox-3 (Kleiner et al., 2007), Palamedes v1.8.0 (Prins & Kingdon, 2009) and Matlab 2012b. 
To quantify the perceived illusory speeds of objects with different sizes, we used a 2AFC paradigm to obtain the point of subjective equality (PSE; Figure 1, Supplementary Movie S1). In each trial, one reference and one test object moving to either the left or right were presented consecutively with one above and the other below the fixation dot. The presentation order of the reference and test object was randomized. Participants were required to answer which object appeared to move faster: the one above or below the fixation dot. If the reference (or test) object was chosen, the test object's speed would increase (or decrease) by 20% of the reference's speed in the next presentation. Ten choice reversals were required for the experiment termination of each pair of objects. The last six reversal points were averaged to determine the PSE. Participants' head position was fixed during the experiment with a chin rest. 
Figure 1
 
Schematic procedure of quantifying the speed–size illusion. The 2° reference object and one of the four test objects (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, 3°) moving either to the left or right were shown consecutively with a 400-ms fixation period in between. One object was located above and the other below the fixation dot. Objects traversed 6° visual angle. Participants were required to answer whether the object above or below appeared to move faster by pressing a key corresponding to the spatial location. In Experiment 1a, the distance between the objects' centers and the horizontal line (not part of the stimulus) passing the fixation dot was kept constant. In Experiment 1b, the objects' inner edges rather than centers were equidistant from the middle line. Images are not to scale. See Supplementary Movie S1 for demonstration of the procedure.
Figure 1
 
Schematic procedure of quantifying the speed–size illusion. The 2° reference object and one of the four test objects (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, 3°) moving either to the left or right were shown consecutively with a 400-ms fixation period in between. One object was located above and the other below the fixation dot. Objects traversed 6° visual angle. Participants were required to answer whether the object above or below appeared to move faster by pressing a key corresponding to the spatial location. In Experiment 1a, the distance between the objects' centers and the horizontal line (not part of the stimulus) passing the fixation dot was kept constant. In Experiment 1b, the objects' inner edges rather than centers were equidistant from the middle line. Images are not to scale. See Supplementary Movie S1 for demonstration of the procedure.
The reference object subtended 2° visual angle and moved at one of the five possible speeds (7°–35°/s, 7°/s increments). The sizes of the test objects were 1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, and 3°. For each pair of objects (5 speeds × 4 test sizes), the test object's initial speed was 40% or 160% of the reference' speed (low/high initial speed). In total, there were 40 distinct pairs of objects. The experiment was divided into two blocks, one with the 1° and 2.5° test objects and the other with the 1.5° and 3° test objects. Half of the participants finished the 1° and 2.5° block first. Trials within each block were mixed and randomly presented. In Experiment 1a, the reference and test objects' centers were equidistant from the middle horizontal line where the fixation dot was located. In Experiment 1b, the objects' inner edges were equidistant from the horizontal line. 
One participant in Experiment 1b was excluded from the analysis due to inconsistent responses that resulted in failure of convergence between the low and high initial speed conditions (>40% difference on average). Three and two participants in Experiments 2a and 2b, respectively, were excluded from the analysis due to the same reason. 
Simulation
We obtained approximation of retinal image speed distributions for different image sizes by simulation. As described in previous studies (Wojtach, Sung, & Purves, 2009; Wojtach, Sung, Truong, & Purves, 2008), a virtual sphere with 3-D objects in translational motion was used to simulate the retinal image formation process (Figure 2). Objects were initiated outside a frustum and projected onto a 50° × 50° image plane once they entered the frustum. The virtual environment's size was defined in arbitrary units such that the image plane measured 50 × 50 units and the sphere radius was 230 units. 
Figure 2
 
Schematic drawing of the virtual environment used in simulation. 3-D objects were initiated inside the sphere and outside the frustum with a random initial position and moving direction. If an object entered the frustum, it would project onto the image plane (blue square), and its projected size, speed, and trajectory would be recorded. Projected images that matched test objects' sizes and passed a 6° trajectory on the image plane were analyzed for their speed occurrence frequency. Image was not to scale. See Methods for more detail.
Figure 2
 
Schematic drawing of the virtual environment used in simulation. 3-D objects were initiated inside the sphere and outside the frustum with a random initial position and moving direction. If an object entered the frustum, it would project onto the image plane (blue square), and its projected size, speed, and trajectory would be recorded. Projected images that matched test objects' sizes and passed a 6° trajectory on the image plane were analyzed for their speed occurrence frequency. Image was not to scale. See Methods for more detail.
The speed of the objects ranged from 0.1 to 150 units/s, and the size ranged from 0.1 to 6.0 units, following uniform distribution. The speed of the objects would give rise to the maximum image speed of 150°/s, roughly the limit of human perception (Burr & Ross, 1986). The sizes of objects were restricted to the relatively small range to generate projected images with size relevant to the psychophysical experiment (1°–3°, Experiment 1). As discussed in the Results section, simulation result is not affected much by a specific range or distribution of object speed and size. 
To show the simulation is biologically relevant, we modified the above-described simulation such that real-world data could be incorporated into the second simulation. One set of biologically relevant parameters was terrestrial mammal size and speed. Because species density, distribution, motion pattern, and all other data that are related to retinal motion statistics were not immediately clear, we adopted uniform speed and size distribution as a general estimation. In this simulation, the virtual environment was defined by physical distance. The image plane was defined to be 5 × 5 mm, on the order of fovea size. The objects' minimum size was 0.01 m, and the maximum was 5 m, roughly the size of the largest terrestrial mammal. Objects' speed ranged from 0.01 to 16 m/s. The speed for different sizes was confined to be the same range, imitating the tendency in different-sized mammals (Garland, 1983). 
To obtain speed distributions, projected trajectories that traversed 6° on the image plane were analyzed. The sampling resolution was 0.1°, meaning that a 7° trajectory would be sampled 10 times because it contained 10 pairs of starting and ending positions that would result in a 6° projection. Because linear motion in 3-D space did not necessarily generate uniform size and speed on the image plane, the average speed and midpoint size were calculated. Speed distributions were created by compiling the number of occurrences of a range of speed for 1° ± 0.05°, 1.5° ± 0.075°, 2.5° ± 0.125°, and 3° ± 0.15° retinal images, corresponding to the object sizes (±5%) used in psychophysical experiments. 
Training (Experiment 2)
Twenty-seven (14 female, 19–32 years old) and 25 (11 female, 19–27 years old) naive participants with normal or corrected-to-normal vision took part in Experiment 2a and b, respectively. Informed consent was obtained, and the participants were reimbursed S$15. The experimental setup was the same as Experiment 1. 
The speed–size illusion was quantified before, immediately after, and 10 min after the training session with the same procedure as in Experiment 1 except that only 1° and 3° objects were tested against a 2° reference object moving at 21°/s. Between the first and second post-training tests, participants remained in the laboratory. During the training session, participants watched a 500-s movie containing 30 moving white disks whose speed and size changed constantly (Supplementary Movie S2 and S3 for Experiment 2a and b, respectively). The objects followed laws of reflection when they hit the screen boundaries. 
In Experiment 2a, the objects' size changed between 0.5° and 3.5°, and speed changed between 7°/s and 35°/s. Their size and speed were inversely correlated in a linear fashion (i.e., an object moved at 35°/s when it was 0.5° in size and moved at 7°/s when it was 3.5° in size). In Experiment 2b, the same size and speed ranges applied, but they were positively correlated. Objects were initiated with random sizes, moving directions, and locations within the display. When the movie began, half of the randomly selected objects started to enlarge, and the other half shrunk at the rate of ∼0.72°/s (0.5 pixel/frame) while their speeds changed accordingly. When the objects became maximum or minimum in size, the change reversed. 
The speed–size distribution in Experiment 2a's training movie was against the distribution observed in the simulation that mimicked natural statistics, in which smaller objects tended to move slower. It was therefore an “unnatural” distribution because it violated the trend in the environment. The reverse was true for the “natural” distribution in Experiment 2b. To keep participants engaged in viewing, one randomly selected object would become brighter about every 3 s on average, and the participants were required to detect that object by pressing a key as quickly and accurately as possible. 
Results
Quantifying the speed–size illusion
As shown in Figure 3A, larger objects possessed higher speeds when they were deemed to move equally fast as smaller ones across the tested spectrum of size and speed. Conversely, smaller objects appeared to move faster when their actual speeds were the same as larger objects. The speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference (representing the effect size of the illusion) across the five speeds are shown in Figure 3B. A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test examining the effect of size and speed on the ratios revealed a significant effect of size, F(3, 51) = 133.77, p < Display Formula\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\({10^{ - 10}}\), and an interaction between size and speed, F(12, 204) = 2.13, p = 0.017, but no significant effect of speed, F(4, 68) = 0.19, p = 0.94. Post hoc paired-sample t tests confirmed the main effect that larger objects had higher ratios: 1° and 1.5°, t(89) = −3.9, p = 1.8 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 4}}\); 1.5° and 2.5°, t(89) = −9.9, p = 6.1 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 16}}\); 2.5° and 3°, t(89) = −3.9, p = 1.6 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 4}}\), and the interaction effect that only the slowest speed (7°/s) resulted in significantly higher ratios when compared to any other speeds: 7°/s and 14°/s, t(71) = 5.1, p = 2.7Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 6}}\); 14°/s and 21°/s, t(71) = 0.1, p = 0.92; 21°/s and 28°/s, t(71) = 1.0, p = 0.30; 28°/s and 35°/s, t(71) = 0.7, p = 0.48. 
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1a. (A) Smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effect of size, F(3, 51) = 133.77, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\), and interaction between size and speed, F(12, 204) = 2.13, p = 0.017, but no significant effect of speed, F(4, 68) = 0.19, p = 0.94. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1a. (A) Smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effect of size, F(3, 51) = 133.77, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\), and interaction between size and speed, F(12, 204) = 2.13, p = 0.017, but no significant effect of speed, F(4, 68) = 0.19, p = 0.94. Error bars indicate standard error.
We also examined the possibility that this effect was driven by the varying distances between fixation dot and the objects' inner edge because larger objects had some part being closer to the fixation dot in Experiment 1a, and it might engage more foveal vision (Tynan & Sekuler, 1982). Hence, the edge-fixation distance was matched for all testing objects in Experiment 1b. Similar to Experiment 1a, larger objects had higher speeds when they were perceived to be equally fast as smaller ones (Figure 4A). A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test examining the effect of size and speed on the ratios revealed a significant effect of size, F(3, 42) = 91.85, p < Display Formula\({10^{ - 10}}\), and speed, F(4, 56) = 2.56, p = 0.048, and an interaction between them, F(12, 168) = 4.23, p Display Formula\( = \) 7.9Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 6}}\). Post hoc paired-sample t tests confirmed the main effect that larger objects had higher ratios: 1° and 1.5°, t(74) = −5.5, p = 5.0 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 7}}\); 1.5° and 2.5°, t(74) = −8.3, p = 3.5 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 12}}\); 2.5° and 3°, t(74) = −3.8, p = 3.2 Display Formula\( \times {10^{ - 4}}\)), and the mixed effects of speed, p = 0.09, 0.012, 0.0062, and 0.013 for 7°/s (and 14°/s, 21°/s, 28°/s, 35°/s); p = 0.11, 0.048, and 0.27 for 14°/s (and 21°/s, 28°/s, 35°/s); p = 0.93 and 0.73 for 21°/s (and 28°/s, 35°/s); and p = 0.63 for 28°/s and 35°/s. The results therefore confirmed that the speed misperception did not originate from different edge-fixation distances. 
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 1b. (A) Similar to Experiment 1a, smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test of the ratios revealed a significant effect of speed, F(4, 56) = 2.56, p = 0.048; size, F(3, 42) = 91.85, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\); and interaction between them, F(12, 168) = 4.23, p \( = \) 7.9 \( \times {10^{ - 6}}\). Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 1b. (A) Similar to Experiment 1a, smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test of the ratios revealed a significant effect of speed, F(4, 56) = 2.56, p = 0.048; size, F(3, 42) = 91.85, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\); and interaction between them, F(12, 168) = 4.23, p \( = \) 7.9 \( \times {10^{ - 6}}\). Error bars indicate standard error.
Simulation
The speed probability density function for five image sizes (1°–3°) are plotted in Figure 5. In both the general 3-D environment (Figure 5A and C) and biological data–inspired model (Figure 5B and D), the distribution for smaller images appeared to fall off more quickly, reflecting the fact that small images often resulted from objects in far distance and, hence, also had smaller projected speeds. The overall projected speed in the second simulation was lower because biological motions are relatively slow. Figure 5C and D shows the speed probability for image sizes from 0.1° to 5°, in which the pixel intensity represented probability. The subfigures above had the probability normalized to the most probable speed's probability within each individual size (0.1° resolution), and the subfigures below had the probability normalized to the overall maximum. The distributions were highly skewed to low speed and small size. 
Figure 5
 
Speed distribution for different image sizes obtained from simulation. Smaller objects' speed distributions fall off more quickly. (A, C) Speed distributions obtained from general simulation. (B, D) Speed distribution obtained from biologically inspired model. (C, D) Pixel intensity–represented probability. The subfigures above had probability normalized within each size, and the subfigures below had probability normalized to the overall maximum.
Figure 5
 
Speed distribution for different image sizes obtained from simulation. Smaller objects' speed distributions fall off more quickly. (A, C) Speed distributions obtained from general simulation. (B, D) Speed distribution obtained from biologically inspired model. (C, D) Pixel intensity–represented probability. The subfigures above had probability normalized within each size, and the subfigures below had probability normalized to the overall maximum.
Because real-world motion statistics are not well defined, we have assumed uniform distributions for the objects' speed and size in our simulation. The result that smaller images' speed distribution fell off more quickly, however, is mostly determined by the geometrical transformation from 3-D to 2-D motion rather than the specific parameters being used. To see the point, we can compare the speed distributions between two retinal image sizes α and β, where α is smaller than β (Figure 6). α results from projections of objects from size x to z, and β results from projections of objects from size y to z, where x < y < z, and z is the maximum object size in the environment. Because α is smaller, the majority of the sources that give rise to α (objects from size y to z) will tend to be further away when comparing to the sources that give rise to β, and therefore, these objects' projected image speeds are also lower for α. Assuming further that objects are homogeneously distributed in the environment, then images of size α will be mostly generated by objects in the [y, z] range rather than the [x, y] range because the number of occurrences for any object size is proportional to the distance from viewpoint. Smaller image speeds (relative to β's) will then dominate α's speed distribution. Thus, the speed distribution for α will always fall off more quickly comparing to β unless objects in the [x, y] range are much more abundant and much faster than objects in the [y, z] range. Taken together, smaller objects' speed distributions falling off quickly is likely the natural result of geometrical transformation. 
Figure 6
 
Smaller retinal images are mostly projections of objects in far distance. α < β, x < y < z, and z is the maximum object size. The majority of objects that generate retinal images of size α (objects in the size range [y, z]) are further away comparing to objects of the same size that generate retinal images of size β. These objects also generate lower image speeds for α than β regardless of the their speed and size distribution. If objects of various sizes are homogeneously distributed in the environment, images of size α are mostly generated by objects in the [y, z] range rather than objects in the [x, y] range. Thus, α is likely to have speed distribution fall off more quickly comparing to β.
Figure 6
 
Smaller retinal images are mostly projections of objects in far distance. α < β, x < y < z, and z is the maximum object size. The majority of objects that generate retinal images of size α (objects in the size range [y, z]) are further away comparing to objects of the same size that generate retinal images of size β. These objects also generate lower image speeds for α than β regardless of the their speed and size distribution. If objects of various sizes are homogeneously distributed in the environment, images of size α are mostly generated by objects in the [y, z] range rather than objects in the [x, y] range. Thus, α is likely to have speed distribution fall off more quickly comparing to β.
Training
In Experiment 2a and b, the speed–size illusion persisted before and after the training session. As shown in Figure 7, 1° objects had lower image speed, and 3° objects had higher image speed when they were deemed to be equally fast as the 2° reference. The illusory speed perception was reduced, t(23) = −2.1, p = 0.043 for 1° object and 2° reference, t(23) = Display Formula\(\)2.3, p = 0.03 for 3° object and 2° reference, paired-sample t test, after unnatural speed–size distribution training in Experiment 2a (Figure 7A and C, Post 1) when participants viewed slow-moving large objects and fast-moving small objects. Conversely, the illusion did not attenuate after training with natural speed–size distribution in Experiment 2b (Figure 7B and D, Post 1). It was observed that 10 min after the training session, the illusory effect was neither significantly different from that before the training, nor significantly different from the first post-test (Figure 7, Post 2), suggesting the existence of some residue effect. Note that the reported p values were not corrected for multiple comparisons (number of comparisons = 6). 
Figure 7
 
Speed ratios between 1° or 3° objects and 2° reference when they were perceived to be equally fast. (A, B) Bars within the 25th and 75th percentile boxes represent median. (C, D) Each circle/diamond represents data from one participant. Data from the same participant are connected by lines. Short horizontal bars represent group means. (A, C) Unnatural speed–size distribution training. The illusion reduced immediately after the training, t(23) = −2.1, p = 0.043 for 1° object; t(23) = 2.3, p = 0.030 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 1, and recovered 10 min after the training, t(23) = −0.6, p = 0.57 for 1° object; t(23) = 1.6, p = 0.12 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. (B, D) Natural speed–size distribution training. The illusion persisted both immediately after the training, t(22) =1.3, p = 0.21 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.3, p = 0.22 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post1, and 10 min after the training, t(22) = 0.5, p = 0.65 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.4, p = 0.18 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. Post 1 and Post 2 ratios were not significantly different for both trainings. Unnatural distribution: t(23) = −1.7, p = 0.10 for 1° object; t(23) = −0.03, p = 0.98 for 3° object. Natural distribution: t(22) = 0.7, p = 0.51 for 1° object; t(22) = −0.06, p = 0.96 for 3° object. Pre: Test before training. Post 1: Test immediately after training. Post 2: Test 10 mins after training. P values were uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
Figure 7
 
Speed ratios between 1° or 3° objects and 2° reference when they were perceived to be equally fast. (A, B) Bars within the 25th and 75th percentile boxes represent median. (C, D) Each circle/diamond represents data from one participant. Data from the same participant are connected by lines. Short horizontal bars represent group means. (A, C) Unnatural speed–size distribution training. The illusion reduced immediately after the training, t(23) = −2.1, p = 0.043 for 1° object; t(23) = 2.3, p = 0.030 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 1, and recovered 10 min after the training, t(23) = −0.6, p = 0.57 for 1° object; t(23) = 1.6, p = 0.12 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. (B, D) Natural speed–size distribution training. The illusion persisted both immediately after the training, t(22) =1.3, p = 0.21 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.3, p = 0.22 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post1, and 10 min after the training, t(22) = 0.5, p = 0.65 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.4, p = 0.18 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. Post 1 and Post 2 ratios were not significantly different for both trainings. Unnatural distribution: t(23) = −1.7, p = 0.10 for 1° object; t(23) = −0.03, p = 0.98 for 3° object. Natural distribution: t(22) = 0.7, p = 0.51 for 1° object; t(22) = −0.06, p = 0.96 for 3° object. Pre: Test before training. Post 1: Test immediately after training. Post 2: Test 10 mins after training. P values were uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
Discussion
Our results show that the size of an object affects the perception of its speed. In particular, smaller objects appeared to move faster in translational motion. This phenomenon is correlated with the observation that smaller retinal images have speed distributions fall off more quickly, which is likely the natural result of 3-D to 2-D transformation—distant objects generate smaller retinal image size and speed. Viewing a movie containing unnatural speed–size statistics tended to modulate the illusion in a way that is consistent with the speed distribution bias. These results demonstrated that speed perception is correlated with retinal image statistics although how and why they were linked is not immediately clear. There are a few theories that might bridge this gap, including empirical ranking theory, Bayesian theory, and visual adaptation (Soon, Dubey, Ananyev, & Hsieh, 2017). We discuss their relevance to our results below. 
The empirical ranking theory proposes that inference of physical property is not the goal of the visual system; rather, visual experience is defined by the trial-and-error evolutionary history of the visual system's interaction with the world (Purves, Morgenstern, & Wojtach, 2015; Purves, Wojtach, & Lotto, 2011). It postulates that the perceptual quality of a retinal stimulus (e.g., length) is a function of the relative frequency of occurrence of the relevant parameter in accumulated past experiences of a given visual system (Howe & Purves, 2005); perceptual response could be estimated from the cumulative distribution function (CDF) of the parameter. This postulation is similar to the observations based on efficient coding theory. For instance, the contrast response functions of fly compound eye interneurons (Laughlin, 1981) and macaque LGN M cells (Clatworthy, Chirimuuta, Lauritzen, & Tolhurst, 2003) approximate the CDFs of natural scene contrast levels, which is the most efficient way to code contrast with a limited dynamic range. 
If we plot the CDF of speed from the speed distribution functions in Figure 5A, the speed perception of different sizes will be a function of their percentile rank of the image speed (Figure 8A). As shown in Figure 8A, smaller images occupy a higher percentile rank for any image speed. According to the empirical ranking theory, higher percentile rank indicates higher perceived speed (Wojtach et al., 2009), which is consistent with the result. A prediction of Experiment 1's result can be made by getting the image speeds for different sizes at the same percentile rank. It is observed that the speed–size illusion is predicted by the theory based on the image statistics (Figure 8B). In Experiment 2a, the speed–size distribution in the training movie should reduce the difference between the CDFs because small images are biased to higher speed. The training should therefore lead to a reduced illusory effect, according to the theory. This was likely the case immediately after the training (Figure 7A, Pre-Post 1). The fact that this reduction disappeared after 10 min (Figure 7, Pre-Post 2) suggests that temporary changes in the motion statistics (training movie in this case) are not sufficient to induce long-term changes in the visual system's internalized empirical rankings, which are supposed to be shaped in the evolutionary time scale. In summary, the correlation between motion statistics and speed perception is consistent with the empirical ranking theory. 
Figure 8
 
Cumulative distributions of speed and predictions of Experiment 1's result based on empirical ranking theory. (A) The CDF replotted from Figure 5A. Smaller images occupied higher percentile rank for any given image speed. (B) Predicted illusory effect by assuming that the same percentile rank on the CDF is indicative of the same perceived speed. The image speeds of the four test object sizes (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, and 3°) at the same percentile rank (the 2° reference's percentile ranks at image speed 7°/s–35°/s) are the predicted PSEs.
Figure 8
 
Cumulative distributions of speed and predictions of Experiment 1's result based on empirical ranking theory. (A) The CDF replotted from Figure 5A. Smaller images occupied higher percentile rank for any given image speed. (B) Predicted illusory effect by assuming that the same percentile rank on the CDF is indicative of the same perceived speed. The image speeds of the four test object sizes (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, and 3°) at the same percentile rank (the 2° reference's percentile ranks at image speed 7°/s–35°/s) are the predicted PSEs.
Another framework of visual perception that links to natural scene statistics is based on Bayes' theorem (Kersten, Mamassian, & Yuille, 2004). Visual perception in this regard is viewed as a process of probabilistic inference based on current observation and prior knowledge (i.e., prior and likelihood function). Previous works suggest that speed prior is skewed to low speed, and higher measurement noise leads to stronger bias toward the prior (Stocker & Simoncelli, 2006; Weiss et al., 2002). In this account, the speed–size illusion would arise from lower measurement noise for smaller objects, which would then be less biased toward low speed. However, large objects are likely to have less noise because more evidence about speed is available at the edge, especially in the context of Experiment 1, in which objects had high contrast on the black background (Figure 1). 
Another Bayesian explanation is that small objects have speed distributions (i.e., prior) skewed to the higher end comparing to large objects, and hence, the perceived small object motions are biased to higher speed. Although it is true that smaller objects move faster in some cases (e.g., a hunting cheetah is faster than a walking elephant), this is not true in general (e.g., a bicycle is slower than a car). Validating this Bayesian explanation would nevertheless require the speed distribution of objects in the 3-D world to be determined in future experiments. Our simulation relied on generic 3-D world speed and size distributions and, therefore, could not provide evidence for this account. 
The third Bayesian explanation is more specific to the speed–size effect manifested in Experiment 1 but not the effect in general. An observer might implicitly assume that the white disks seen during the experiments were identical objects at different distances. In this scenario, a smaller disk was most likely the object at further distance. Accordingly, it should also possess higher physical speed given the same image speed as a larger disk. An observer who is trying to infer the object's physical speed instead of the disk's image speed would then be biased by the likelihood function P (Physical speed|Image speed) under the constraint of identical objects. This explanation does not apply to the speed–size effect in other cases as the effect often happens between different objects (Clark et al., 2013). 
The results of Experiment 2 do not support the conventional Bayesian explanation. Adams, Graf, and Ernst (2004) showed that the light-from-above prior can be modulated by recent sensory experience. Similarly, viewing small objects moving faster (Experiment 2a) should bias the prior to high speed, which is not observed (Figure 7A). One possibility is that prior update happens at a longer time scale and requires prolonged exposure to novel statistics, far exceeding the training provided in the current experiment. Future investigation is therefore required to determine if the speed–size prior can be modulated with longer training. 
Visual adaptation could also lead to misjudgment of speed and motion direction (Clifford & Ibbotson, 2002). It is conceivable that the constant presence of a certain adaptor will cause an illusory perception to last indefinitely. Hietanen, Crowder, and Ibbotson (2008) showed that adaptation to low speed and test at high speed would increase perceived speed and vice versa. It is then possible that our visual system adapts to the statistically lower speed of small objects, and the speed perception of small objects is recalibrated relative to that of large objects such that small objects appear faster. This explanation requires speed adaptation to be size-specific, similar to the orientation-specific color adaptation seen in the McCollough effect (McCollough, 1965). That is, the adaptation to small objects' relative low speed and large objects' relative high speed leads to separate calibration of small and large objects' speed perception. This concept of constant adaptation is markedly different from conventional adaptation, which is a temporary effect. It is more akin to the concept of Bayesian prior working in the opposite way. 
Experiment 2 falls within the time range of conventional adaptation. The training movies worked essentially as adaptors, and the post-training tests gauged the adaptation effect. Experiment 2a showed adaptation results (Figure 7A) similar to Hietanen et al. (2008), in which adaptation to low speed and test at high speed increased perceived speed and vice versa. Although only significant before correction, the adaptation effect for both low and high speed agreed with Hietanen et al. immediately after the training and then attenuated in minutes. Experiment 2b, however, did not show a significant adaptation effect. The major difference between Hietanen et al.'s and our experiment was that the adaptors in our experiment were a mixture of objects with varying speed and size rather than a fixed size. Specifically, fast and slow adaptors were combined in a single adaptation session, and speed was correlated with size. The results therefore suggest that speed adaptation could be size-specific. 
Conclusion
We showed that the speed-size illusion is correlated with retinal-level speed–size distribution bias. Exposure to biased motion statistics tended to modulate the illusion in the manner that was consistent with the retinal image speed distribution bias. These results could be consistent with empirical ranking theory, Bayesian theory, and motion adaptation. 
Acknowledgments
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Po-Jang Hsieh. 
Address: Neuroscience and Brain Disorders Program, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore. 
References
Adams, W. J., Graf, E. W., & Ernst, M. O. (2004). Experience can change the “light-from-above” prior. Nature Neuroscience, 7 (10), 1057–1058, doi:10.1038/nn1312.
Banks, M. S., Sprague, W. W., Schmoll, J., Parnell, J. A. Q., & Love, G. D. (2015). Why do animal eyes have pupils of different shapes? Science Advances, 1 (7), e1500391–e1500391, doi:10.1126/sciadv.1500391.
Barton, J. E. (2006). A 3D computer simulation test of the Leibowitz hypothesis. Test, 3261, 1–10.
Blakemore, M. R., & Snowden, R. J. (1999). The effect of contrast upon perceived speed: A general phenomenon? Perception, 28 (1), 33–48, doi:10.1068/p2722.
Burr, D., & Ross, J. (1986). Visual processing of motion. Trends in Neurosciences, 9, 304–307.
Clark, H. E., Perrone, J. A., & Isler, R. B. (2013). Short communication: An illusory size-speed bias and railway crossing collisions. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 55, 226–231, doi:10.1016/j.aap.2013.02.037.
Clatworthy, P. L., Chirimuuta, M., Lauritzen, J. S., & Tolhurst, D. J. (2003). Coding of the contrasts in natural images by populations of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1). Vision Research, 43 (18), 1983–2001, doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00277-3.
Clifford, C. W. G., & Ibbotson, M. R. (2002). Fundamental mechanisms of visual motion detection: Models, cells and functions. Progress in Neurobiology, 68 (6), 409–437.
Distler, H. K., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Van Veen, H. A. H. C., & Hawken, M. J. (2000). Velocity constancy in a virtual reality environment. Perception, 29 (12), 1423–1435, doi:10.1068/p3115.
Garland, T. (1983). The relation between maximal running speed and body mass in terrestrial mammals. Journal of Zoology, 199 (2), 157–170.
Geisler, W. S. (2008). Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. Annual Review of Psychology, 59 (1), 167–192, doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085632.
Girshick, A. R., Landy, M. S., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics. Nature Neuroscience, 14 (7), 926–932, doi:10.1038/nn.2831.
Hietanen, M. A., Crowder, N. A., & Ibbotson, M. R. (2008). Differential changes in human perception of speed due to motion adaptation. Journal of Vision, 8 (11): 6, 1–10, doi:10.1167/8.11.6. [PubMed] [Article]
Howe, C. Q., & Purves, D. (2005). The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image-source relationships. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102 (4), 1234–1239, doi:10.1073/pnas.0409314102.
Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., & Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Annual Review of Psychology, 55 (1), 271–304, http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., Pelli, D., Ingling, A., Murray, R., & Broussard, C. (2007). What's new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception, 36 (14), 1.
Land, M. F., & Nilsson, D.-E. (2012). Animal eyes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Laughlin, S. (1981). A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron's information capacity. Zeitschrift Fur Naturforschung - Section C Journal of Biosciences, 36 (9–10), 910–912, doi:10.1515/znc-1981-9-1040.
Leibowitz, H. W. (1985). Grade crossing accidents and human factors engineering: How a discipline combining technology and behavioral science can help reduce traffic fatalities. American Scientist, 73 (6), 558–562.
Liu, Y., Bovik, A., & Cormack, L. (2008). Disparity statistics in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 8 (11)19: 1–14; doi:10.1167/8.11.19. [PubMed] [Article]
McCollough, C. (1965, Sept 3). Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the human visual system. Science, 149 (3688), 1115–1116.
Mckee, S. P., & Smallman, H. S. (1998). Size and speed constancy. In Walsh V. & Kulikowski J. (Eds.), Perceptual constancy (pp. 373–408). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Prins, N., & Kingdon, F. A. A. (2009). Palamedes: Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data. Available at http://www.palamedestoolbox.org
Purves, D., Morgenstern, Y., & Wojtach, W. T. (2015). Will understanding vision require a wholly empirical paradigm? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–6, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01072.
Purves, D., Wojtach, W. T., & Lotto, R. B. (2011). Colloquium paper: Understanding vision in wholly empirical terms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108, 15588–15595, doi:10.1073/pnas.1012178108.
Soon, C. S., Dubey, R., Ananyev, E., & Hsieh, P.-J. (2017). Approaches to understanding visual illusions. In Zhao Q. (Ed.), Computational and cognitive neuroscience of vision (pp. 221–233). Singapore: Springer Singapore, doi:10.1007/978-981-10-0213-7_10.
Stocker, A. A., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2006). Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception. Nature Neuroscience, 9 (4), 578–585, doi:10.1038/nn1669.
Tynan, P. D., & Sekuler, R. (1982). Motion processing in peripheral vision: Reaction time and perceived velocity. Vision Research, 22 (1), 61–68.
Webster, M. A. (2015). Visual adaptation. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1 (1), 547–569, doi:10.1007/bf00535812.
Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E. P., & Adelson, E. H. (2002). Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nature Neuroscience, 5 (6), 598–604, doi:10.1038/nn858.
Wojtach, W. T., Sung, K., & Purves, D. (2009). An empirical explanation of the speed-distance effect. PLoS ONE, 4 (8), 36771, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006771.
Wojtach, W. T., Sung, K., Truong, S., & Purves, D. (2008). An empirical explanation of the flash-lag effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 105, 16338–16343, doi:10.1073/pnas.0808916105.
Yang, Z., & Purves, D. (2003). A statistical explanation of visual space. Nature Neuroscience, 6 (6), 632–640, doi:10.1038/nn1059.
Figure 1
 
Schematic procedure of quantifying the speed–size illusion. The 2° reference object and one of the four test objects (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, 3°) moving either to the left or right were shown consecutively with a 400-ms fixation period in between. One object was located above and the other below the fixation dot. Objects traversed 6° visual angle. Participants were required to answer whether the object above or below appeared to move faster by pressing a key corresponding to the spatial location. In Experiment 1a, the distance between the objects' centers and the horizontal line (not part of the stimulus) passing the fixation dot was kept constant. In Experiment 1b, the objects' inner edges rather than centers were equidistant from the middle line. Images are not to scale. See Supplementary Movie S1 for demonstration of the procedure.
Figure 1
 
Schematic procedure of quantifying the speed–size illusion. The 2° reference object and one of the four test objects (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, 3°) moving either to the left or right were shown consecutively with a 400-ms fixation period in between. One object was located above and the other below the fixation dot. Objects traversed 6° visual angle. Participants were required to answer whether the object above or below appeared to move faster by pressing a key corresponding to the spatial location. In Experiment 1a, the distance between the objects' centers and the horizontal line (not part of the stimulus) passing the fixation dot was kept constant. In Experiment 1b, the objects' inner edges rather than centers were equidistant from the middle line. Images are not to scale. See Supplementary Movie S1 for demonstration of the procedure.
Figure 2
 
Schematic drawing of the virtual environment used in simulation. 3-D objects were initiated inside the sphere and outside the frustum with a random initial position and moving direction. If an object entered the frustum, it would project onto the image plane (blue square), and its projected size, speed, and trajectory would be recorded. Projected images that matched test objects' sizes and passed a 6° trajectory on the image plane were analyzed for their speed occurrence frequency. Image was not to scale. See Methods for more detail.
Figure 2
 
Schematic drawing of the virtual environment used in simulation. 3-D objects were initiated inside the sphere and outside the frustum with a random initial position and moving direction. If an object entered the frustum, it would project onto the image plane (blue square), and its projected size, speed, and trajectory would be recorded. Projected images that matched test objects' sizes and passed a 6° trajectory on the image plane were analyzed for their speed occurrence frequency. Image was not to scale. See Methods for more detail.
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1a. (A) Smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effect of size, F(3, 51) = 133.77, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\), and interaction between size and speed, F(12, 204) = 2.13, p = 0.017, but no significant effect of speed, F(4, 68) = 0.19, p = 0.94. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1a. (A) Smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant effect of size, F(3, 51) = 133.77, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\), and interaction between size and speed, F(12, 204) = 2.13, p = 0.017, but no significant effect of speed, F(4, 68) = 0.19, p = 0.94. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 1b. (A) Similar to Experiment 1a, smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test of the ratios revealed a significant effect of speed, F(4, 56) = 2.56, p = 0.048; size, F(3, 42) = 91.85, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\); and interaction between them, F(12, 168) = 4.23, p \( = \) 7.9 \( \times {10^{ - 6}}\). Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 1b. (A) Similar to Experiment 1a, smaller objects had lower image speed when they were perceived to be equally fast as the reference. Yellow dots represent the reference object's speed. (B) The PSE speed ratios between test objects and the 2° reference. A two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test of the ratios revealed a significant effect of speed, F(4, 56) = 2.56, p = 0.048; size, F(3, 42) = 91.85, p < \({10^{ - 10}}\); and interaction between them, F(12, 168) = 4.23, p \( = \) 7.9 \( \times {10^{ - 6}}\). Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 5
 
Speed distribution for different image sizes obtained from simulation. Smaller objects' speed distributions fall off more quickly. (A, C) Speed distributions obtained from general simulation. (B, D) Speed distribution obtained from biologically inspired model. (C, D) Pixel intensity–represented probability. The subfigures above had probability normalized within each size, and the subfigures below had probability normalized to the overall maximum.
Figure 5
 
Speed distribution for different image sizes obtained from simulation. Smaller objects' speed distributions fall off more quickly. (A, C) Speed distributions obtained from general simulation. (B, D) Speed distribution obtained from biologically inspired model. (C, D) Pixel intensity–represented probability. The subfigures above had probability normalized within each size, and the subfigures below had probability normalized to the overall maximum.
Figure 6
 
Smaller retinal images are mostly projections of objects in far distance. α < β, x < y < z, and z is the maximum object size. The majority of objects that generate retinal images of size α (objects in the size range [y, z]) are further away comparing to objects of the same size that generate retinal images of size β. These objects also generate lower image speeds for α than β regardless of the their speed and size distribution. If objects of various sizes are homogeneously distributed in the environment, images of size α are mostly generated by objects in the [y, z] range rather than objects in the [x, y] range. Thus, α is likely to have speed distribution fall off more quickly comparing to β.
Figure 6
 
Smaller retinal images are mostly projections of objects in far distance. α < β, x < y < z, and z is the maximum object size. The majority of objects that generate retinal images of size α (objects in the size range [y, z]) are further away comparing to objects of the same size that generate retinal images of size β. These objects also generate lower image speeds for α than β regardless of the their speed and size distribution. If objects of various sizes are homogeneously distributed in the environment, images of size α are mostly generated by objects in the [y, z] range rather than objects in the [x, y] range. Thus, α is likely to have speed distribution fall off more quickly comparing to β.
Figure 7
 
Speed ratios between 1° or 3° objects and 2° reference when they were perceived to be equally fast. (A, B) Bars within the 25th and 75th percentile boxes represent median. (C, D) Each circle/diamond represents data from one participant. Data from the same participant are connected by lines. Short horizontal bars represent group means. (A, C) Unnatural speed–size distribution training. The illusion reduced immediately after the training, t(23) = −2.1, p = 0.043 for 1° object; t(23) = 2.3, p = 0.030 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 1, and recovered 10 min after the training, t(23) = −0.6, p = 0.57 for 1° object; t(23) = 1.6, p = 0.12 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. (B, D) Natural speed–size distribution training. The illusion persisted both immediately after the training, t(22) =1.3, p = 0.21 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.3, p = 0.22 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post1, and 10 min after the training, t(22) = 0.5, p = 0.65 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.4, p = 0.18 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. Post 1 and Post 2 ratios were not significantly different for both trainings. Unnatural distribution: t(23) = −1.7, p = 0.10 for 1° object; t(23) = −0.03, p = 0.98 for 3° object. Natural distribution: t(22) = 0.7, p = 0.51 for 1° object; t(22) = −0.06, p = 0.96 for 3° object. Pre: Test before training. Post 1: Test immediately after training. Post 2: Test 10 mins after training. P values were uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
Figure 7
 
Speed ratios between 1° or 3° objects and 2° reference when they were perceived to be equally fast. (A, B) Bars within the 25th and 75th percentile boxes represent median. (C, D) Each circle/diamond represents data from one participant. Data from the same participant are connected by lines. Short horizontal bars represent group means. (A, C) Unnatural speed–size distribution training. The illusion reduced immediately after the training, t(23) = −2.1, p = 0.043 for 1° object; t(23) = 2.3, p = 0.030 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 1, and recovered 10 min after the training, t(23) = −0.6, p = 0.57 for 1° object; t(23) = 1.6, p = 0.12 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. (B, D) Natural speed–size distribution training. The illusion persisted both immediately after the training, t(22) =1.3, p = 0.21 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.3, p = 0.22 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post1, and 10 min after the training, t(22) = 0.5, p = 0.65 for 1° object; t(22) = −1.4, p = 0.18 for 3° object; paired-sample t test between Pre and Post 2. Post 1 and Post 2 ratios were not significantly different for both trainings. Unnatural distribution: t(23) = −1.7, p = 0.10 for 1° object; t(23) = −0.03, p = 0.98 for 3° object. Natural distribution: t(22) = 0.7, p = 0.51 for 1° object; t(22) = −0.06, p = 0.96 for 3° object. Pre: Test before training. Post 1: Test immediately after training. Post 2: Test 10 mins after training. P values were uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
Figure 8
 
Cumulative distributions of speed and predictions of Experiment 1's result based on empirical ranking theory. (A) The CDF replotted from Figure 5A. Smaller images occupied higher percentile rank for any given image speed. (B) Predicted illusory effect by assuming that the same percentile rank on the CDF is indicative of the same perceived speed. The image speeds of the four test object sizes (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, and 3°) at the same percentile rank (the 2° reference's percentile ranks at image speed 7°/s–35°/s) are the predicted PSEs.
Figure 8
 
Cumulative distributions of speed and predictions of Experiment 1's result based on empirical ranking theory. (A) The CDF replotted from Figure 5A. Smaller images occupied higher percentile rank for any given image speed. (B) Predicted illusory effect by assuming that the same percentile rank on the CDF is indicative of the same perceived speed. The image speeds of the four test object sizes (1°, 1.5°, 2.5°, and 3°) at the same percentile rank (the 2° reference's percentile ranks at image speed 7°/s–35°/s) are the predicted PSEs.
Supplement 1
Supplement 2
Supplement 3
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×