The proportion and magnitude of the apparent sway for the experimental and control conditions have been presented in
Figure 3. A repeated measures ANOVA with condition (experimental vs. control) and alternate frequency as independent variables and proportion as a dependent variable revealed that the main effects of condition and alternate frequency were both significant,
F(1, 9) = 73.690,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.891;
F(2, 18) = 16.383,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.645, respectively. More importantly, the interaction was also significant,
F(2, 18) = 16.761,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.651. Next, independent ANOVAs were carried out for each condition, and results showed that, only in the experimental condition, the proportion significantly varied as a function of the alternate frequency,
F(2, 18) = 30.144,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.770, and this was not true for the control condition,
F(2, 18) = 1.198,
p = 0.325, η
2 = 0.117. Therefore, post hoc
t tests were only conducted for the experimental condition. The proportion of the apparent sway was significantly larger at the 1.5- and 3.0-Hz alternations than it was at the 8.57-Hz alternation (
ts > 4,
ps < 0.01) with a marginal significance between 1.5 Hz and 3.0 Hz,
t(9) = 2.803,
p = 0.062. For the experimental condition, significantly more apparent sway was perceived than 50% at the 1.5- and 3.0-Hz alternations,
ts ≥ 3.000,
p < 0.05, while much less apparent sway was perceived than 50% at the fast frequency,
t(9) = −3.613,
p = 0.017, consistent with the observations in Experiment 1. However, for the control condition, significantly less apparent sway was perceived than 50% at all the alternation frequencies (
ts < −3.0,
ps < 0.05).
A similar pattern was found for the magnitude. The two main effects and the interaction were all significant: condition, F(1, 9) = 67.046, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.882; alternate frequency, F(2, 18) = 16.252, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.644; interaction, F(2, 18) = 14.356, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.615. The significant interaction was also attributed to the significant effect of alternate frequency in the experimental condition, F(2, 18) = 19.902, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.689, but not in the control condition, F(2, 18) = 1.702, p = 0.224, η2 = 0.159. For the experimental condition, the 1.5- and 3.0-Hz alternations generated a greater magnitude of the apparent sway than that at the 8.57-Hz alternation was (ts > 3, ps < 0.05), and the difference between 1.5 Hz and 3.0 Hz was marginally significant, t(9) = 2.624, p = 0.083.
The results of Experiment 2 further confirmed that slower alternate frequency instigated and amplified apparent sway, and more importantly, this apparent sway can rarely be perceived when the surround, composed by interweaved clockwise and counterclockwise orientations, alternates. Therefore, the perceived apparent sway caused by the alternation of opposite-oriented contexts indicates a dynamic illusion on the basis of the direct tilt effect. However, this dynamic tilt illusion was only measured by subjective reports until now. Therefore, we required participants to manually alter a truly swaying grating to match the dynamic tilt illusion in Experiment 3. This design allowed us to precisely measure the perceived angles and alternate speed of the apparent sway concurrently in order to compare with the classic tilt illusion.