We first tested whether and how the spatiotemporal properties of dot-array stimuli get integrated and concur in shaping numerical representations.
In the random position change condition, the reference stimulus comprised a portion of dots randomly changing position at different frequencies—that is, disappearing and reappearing in new positions—but keeping the total number of dots constant throughout the duration of the presentation. In a 2 × 3 experimental design, we tested two different proportions of randomly changing stimuli (25% and 50%), and three different temporal frequencies (2, 6, or 8 Hz). For example, a condition of 25% and 6 Hz for a dot array containing 20 dots meant that each of the three times during the 500-ms presentation time, five of the 20 dots disappeared from their locations but five other dots appeared in new locations (
Supplementary Movie 1). The average shift in position underwent by the randomly changing dots was about 5.4° (
SD = 0.57; calculated across a simulation of 10,000 repetitions of the stimuli), which ensured that no apparent motion can be elicited by those shifts in position (i.e., Gepshtein, Tyukin, & Kubovy,
2007; Grossberg & Rudd,
1992). The lack of any motion energy due to the large spatial extent and instantaneous shifts in position assured that randomly changing stimuli represent a distinct class of stimuli compared to the smooth motion condition (see below), and could not be interpreted as extremely fast motion.
In the smooth motion condition, we tested whether motion integration is involved in this stage of analysis. There, static dot array images were compared with a dot arrays comprising a portion of moving dots (50% or 100%) with different speeds (3°/s, 6°/s, or 9°/s), in a 2 × 3 design similar to the random position change condition described above. For example, a condition of 50% and 3°/s for a dot array containing 20 dots meant that 10 of the 20 dots moved in random linear trajectories at a speed of 3°/s. Moving dots were contained within an invisible circle where the dots are drawn; the dots bounced back when they reached the boundary of that invisible circle (
Supplementary Movie 2). Our main prediction about the effect of motion integration in the smooth motion condition concerns the possibility that it might prevent some of the effect provided by the spatiotemporal modulation. Namely, integrating the different positions of the dots across the motion paths might reduce any effect due to the shifts in position of the items. According to this prediction, the parameters of the moving dots were chosen to allow a more conservative comparison with the random position change condition. First, in the smooth motion condition we used larger proportions of moving dots. Furthermore, considering temporal frequency = speed / spatial frequency, and taking two times the size of an individual dot as a measure of spatial frequency (0.8°), the putative temporal frequency of the smooth motion stimuli is slightly higher than the corresponding conditions of the random position change stimuli (3.75, 7.50, or 11.25 Hz, respectively for 3, 6, or 9°/s).
Both conditions were performed separately in blocks and in a random order. In each condition, different combinations of proportion and temporal frequency/speed were randomly intermingled within each block and tested an equal number of times. However, since the dynamic dot arrays are intrinsically more salient compared to the static arrays, response biases could confound the effect of dynamic visual displays (e.g., one could argue that participants just responded to whichever side with more changes). To address this issue, we tested two independent groups of participants with different tasks: While one group was instructed to choose the stimulus containing more dots (judge more task; N = 46 and 47, respectively for the random position change and the smooth motion conditions), the other group was instructed to choose the stimulus containing the smaller amount of dots (judge less task; N = 42 and 44).
Figure 1 shows the results of the different conditions tested in Experiment1, both with the judge more (
Figure 1A) and judge less (
Figure 1B) tasks. First, the random position change stimuli were significantly overestimated compared to the static ones, in all the conditions tested (one-sample
t tests;
t[45] = 7.2,
t[45] = 9.8,
t[45] = 10.9,
t[45] = 9.54,
t[40] = 12.57,
t[45] = 15.07, respectively for the six conditions reported in the leftmost panel of
Figure 1A; all
p values < 0.001). A two-way repeated measure ANOVA with factors proportion (25% and 50%) and temporal frequency (2, 6, 8 Hz) showed a statistically significant main effect of both factors,
F(1, 274) = 62.75,
p < 0.001, and
F(2, 274) = 31.25,
p < 0.001, respectively for proportion and temporal frequency, although with a significant interaction between the two factors,
F(3, 274) = 6.98,
p = 0.002. On the other hand, a post hoc multiple comparisons procedure (Holm-Sidak multiple comparisons) showed that overall the effect increased both as a function of proportion (25% vs. 50%,
t[274] = 7.92,
p < 0.001) and temporal frequency (2 Hz vs. 6 Hz:
t[274] = 4.46,
p < 0.001; 2 Hz vs. 8 Hz:
t[274] = 7.88,
p < 0.001; 6 Hz vs. 8 Hz:
t[274] = 3.42,
p < 0.001).
Smooth motion stimuli were similarly effective, resulting in a significant overestimation of the moving stimulus compared to the static one, t(46) = 6.5, t(46) = 5.5, t(46) = 5.6, t(46) = 3.6, t(46) = 5.6, t(46) = 6.6, all p values < 0.001. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors proportion (50% and 100%) and speed (3°/s, 6°/s and 9°/s) further showed a significant main effect of speed, F(2, 281) = 11.31, p < 0.001, and no main effect of proportion, F(1, 281) = 3.52, p = 0.067, although again with a significant interaction between the two factors, F(3, 281) = 11.06, p < 0.001. A multiple comparisons procedure confirmed that the overall effect does not significantly increase with the proportion of moving dots (50% vs. 100%, t[281] = 1.88, p = 0.067), and showed that while the effect does not significantly increase with speed with the lower proportion (50%) of moving dots (3°/s vs. 6°/s: t[281] = 2.95, p = 0.004; 3°/s vs. 9°/s: t[281] = 6.66, p < 0.001; 6°/s vs. 9°/s: t[281] = 3.71, p < 0.001), it does so with the larger proportion (100%) of moving dots (3°/s vs. 6°/s: t[281] = 0.37, p = 0.915; 3°/s vs. 9°/s: t[281] = 0.05, p = 0.96; 6°/s vs. 9°/s: t[281] = 0.43, p = 0.96).
In order to compare the magnitude of the effects between the random position change and smooth motion conditions, we averaged the effects of all the different combination of temporal frequency/speed and proportion in each subject, and directly compared the distributions of average effects in the two experimental conditions. This test confirmed the stronger overestimation effect in the random position change condition compared to the smooth motion condition (two-sample t test, t[91] = 3.78, one-tailed p value < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.10). The relatively smaller overestimation effect of the smooth motion condition indicates that the integration of spatial and temporal information is dampened by motion processing, suggesting that the motion processing system directly interacts with the numerosity processing system in the brain. However, as the overestimation effect provided by spatiotemporal modulation strongly depends on the parameters chosen for the two conditions—which can be only roughly compared—caution is needed when interpreting the comparison between the two classes of stimuli.
In the judge less task (
Figure 1B), we found a similar pattern of results, with robust and significant changes in perceived numerosity induced by random position change stimuli (one-sample signed rank tests,
Z = 3.5,
Z = 4.8,
Z = 5.0,
Z = 3.6,
Z = 5.2,
Z = 5.6; all
p values < 0.001). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect of both proportion,
F(1, 251) = 24.60,
p < 0.001, and temporal frequency,
F(2, 251) = 25.11,
p < 0.001, and a significant interaction between the two factors,
F(3, 251) = 11.23,
p < 0.001. A multiple comparisons procedure confirmed that the overestimation effect overall increased both as a function of proportion (25% vs. 50%,
t[274] = 4.96,
p < 0.001) and temporal frequency (2 Hz vs. 6 Hz:
t[251] = 4.18,
p < 0.001; 2 Hz vs. 8 Hz:
t[251] = 7.04,
p < 0.001; 6 Hz vs. 8 Hz:
t[251] = 2.86,
p = 0.005; although within the lower proportion, the comparison 6 Hz vs. 8 Hz did not reach significance:
t[251] = 0.25,
p = 0.80). Smooth motion stimuli caused weaker but systematic overestimation effects in most of the conditions, with the exception of the lower speed (3°/s) conditions, which were the only two conditions where there was no significant effect,
Z = 1.5,
p = 0.12;
Z = 2.2,
p = 0.025;
Z = 4.5,
p < 0.001;
Z = 0.27,
p < 0.79;
Z = 4.5,
p < 0.001;
Z = 5.5,
p < 0.001. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA again showed significant main effects of both proportion,
F(1, 263) = 15.24,
p < 0.001, and speed,
F(2, 263) = 54.56,
p < 0.001, and a significant interaction between the two factors,
F(3, 263) = 13.41,
p < 0.001, while multiple comparisons, differently from the results of the smooth motion condition in the judge more task, showed that the effect overall increased both as a function of proportion (50% vs. 100%,
t[263] = 3.90,
p < 0.001) and speed (3°/s vs. 6°/s:
t[263] = 4.12,
p < 0.001; 3°/s vs. 9°/s:
t[263] = 10.37,
p < 0.001; 6°/s vs. 9°/s:
t[263] = 6.25,
p < 0.001; although the comparison 6 Hz vs. 8 Hz within the lower proportion did not reach significance:
t[263] = 1.03,
p = 0.30).
Similarly to the judge more task, we directly compared the random position change and smooth motion condition by averaging in each subject the effects across the six combinations of parameters within each condition. Again, we found a statistically significant difference between the two conditions, t(84) = 1.93, one-tailed p value = 0.029, Cohen's d = 0.59, demonstrating that random position change stimuli caused a significantly stronger overestimation effect than the smooth motion condition.
Finally, we did not observe any substantial difference in precision (WF) across the different tasks and conditions, suggesting that the effect does not critically depend on an increased difficulty in performing the task. On the other hand, the results showed significantly stronger effects in the judge more task, suggesting that response biases partially contributed to the magnitude of the effect (see
Supplementary Materials), also potentially providing ceiling effects in some of the conditions (i.e., the three conditions corresponding to the lower proportion of moving dots in the smooth motion condition with the judge more task, where the effect seems identical independently of speed).