We show that neuronal blur impairs the discrimination of closely spaced light bars because it makes the light bars wider and their separating gaps narrower. At the same time, we show that neuronal blur enlarges tiny light dots presented on dark backgrounds, an effect that is enhanced by the higher contrast sensitivity of the ON pathway (Chichilnisky & Kalmar,
2002; Kremkow et al.,
2014; Zaghloul et al.,
2003) and dark adaptation (Barlow, Fitzhugh, & Kuffler,
1957; Geisler,
1983; Jackson, Owsley, & McGwin,
1999; J. S. Wang, Estevez, Cornwall, & Kefalov,
2009). Because neuronal blur makes light targets appear larger than dark targets, it explains errors in estimating target size that are generally known as
irradiation illusions (Helmholtz,
1867; Kremkow et al.,
2014). At the same time, neuronal blur reduces the salience of light targets on noisy backgrounds by expanding the size of light-background regions and reducing the target/background contrast more for lights than darks (Komban et al.,
2011,
2014). Unlike optical blur, neuronal blur does not affect perfect black–white edges, because it acts by saturating intermediate gray values. However, all edges projected on the retina are surrounded by gray values because they are slightly blurred by the optics of the eye. Therefore, since dark features in an image are less affected by neuronal blur than are light features, it may be advantageous to have a more precise cortical retinotopy for darks than lights (Kremkow et al.,
2016; K. S. Lee et al.,
2016). Moreover, the perception of detail should be more disrupted by blurring dark rather than light features in an image (Sato, Motoyoshi, & Sato,
2016). Finally, neuronal blur enlarges the size of light stimuli making them cover a larger region of the receptive-field surround from ON foveal neurons, a mechanism that could explain why the cortex is OFF dominated at cortical regions representing central vision (Jin et al.,
2008; Yeh et al.,
2009). In addition, because the neuronal blur suppresses ON more than OFF responses to low spatial frequencies, the cortical OFF dominance could be reinforced by the low spatial frequencies that dominate natural scenes (van der Schaaf & van Hateren,
1996) and the optically blurred images formed by the immature infant eye during brain development (Norcia, Tyler, & Hamer,
1990).