Open Access
Article  |   January 2018
Transsaccadic transfer of distortion adaptation in a natural environment
Author Affiliations
  • Selam W. Habtegiorgis
    Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • Katharina Rifai
    Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • Siegfried Wahl
    Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
Journal of Vision January 2018, Vol.18, 13. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/18.1.13
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Selam W. Habtegiorgis, Katharina Rifai, Siegfried Wahl; Transsaccadic transfer of distortion adaptation in a natural environment. Journal of Vision 2018;18(1):13. https://doi.org/10.1167/18.1.13.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Spatially varying distortions in optical elements—for instance prisms and progressive power lenses—modulate the visual world disparately in different visual areas. Saccadic eye movements in such a complexly distorted environment thereby continuously alter the retinal location of the distortions. Yet the visual system achieves perceptual constancy by compensating for distortions irrespective of their retinal relocations at different fixations. Here, we assessed whether the visual system retains its plasticity to distortions across saccades to attain stability. Specifically, we tapped into reference frames of geometric skew-adaptation aftereffects to evaluate the transfer of retinotopic and spatiotopic distortion information across saccades. Adaptation to skew distortion of natural-image content was tested at retinotopic and spatiotopic locations after a saccade was executed between adaptation and test phases. The skew-adaptation information was partially transferred to a new fixation after a saccade. Significant adaptation aftereffects were obtained at both retinotopic and spatiotopic locations. Conceivably, spatiotopic information was used to counterbalance the saccadic retinal shifts of the distortions. Therefore, distortion processing in a natural visual world does not start anew at each fixation; rather, retinotopic and spatiotopic skew information acquired at previous fixations are preserved to mediate stable perception during eye movements.

Introduction
Images on the retina are not an exact replica of the visual world. Instead, they are often distorted by either inherent optical properties of the eye or magnification effects of spectacles worn to correct refractive errors (Fannin & Grosvenor, 2013). In some cases, as in progressive addition lenses and prisms, the magnification axis varies across the surface of the spectacle (Meister & Fisher, 2008). Thus, the geometry of the distortions can even be complexly disparate in different parts of the visual field. On that condition, eye movements shift the retinal location of the distortions and disrupt retinotopic representations. Yet while people adapt to optical elements comprising spatially variable distortions, the visual system seems to compensate for the distortions regardless of their continuous retinal shifts. How this perceptual constancy is attained during eye movements in complexly distorted dynamic natural visual inputs is not well explored. To reveal the underlying visual process, it is vital to assess if visual plasticity to distortion is transferred across saccades in different coordinate systems. 
The visual system retains information of several visual features across a saccade based on their location in retinal and in some cases spatial reference frames (Galletti, Battaglini, & Fattori, 1993; Melcher, 2005, 2007; Melcher & Morrone, 2003; Zimmermann, Morrone, Fink, & Burr, 2013; Zimmermann, Weidner, Abdollahi, & Fink, 2016). The retinotopic location of a stimulus brings perceptual advantages such as in tracking a moving object (Golomb & Kanwisher, 2012b; Howe, Pinto, & Horowitz, 2010). Spatiotopic representation of specific visual features in extraretinal reference frames has been previously demonstrated (Andersen, 1997; Andersen, Essick, & Siegel, 1985; Burr & Morrone, 2011; Crespi et al., 2011; Zimmermann et al., 2013; Zimmermann et al., 2016). The visual system uses spatiotopic information to compensate for saccade-associated retinal shifts. The spatiotopic location of a visual stimulus is therefore important information for successful interaction with the external world (Andersen, 1997; Andersen et al., 1985; Burr & Morrone, 2011). 
Examining the reference frames in which adaptation aftereffects occur is one of the approaches recurrently used to scrutinize transsaccadic transfer of visual information and the underlying coordinate system (Melcher, 2005, 2007; van Boxtel, Alais, & van Ee, 2008; Zimmermann et al., 2016). By introducing an eye movement after adapter presentation and before test-stimulus presentation, it is possible to separately test if any adaptation is retained across a saccade in a retinotopic and a spatiotopic reference frame. In retinotopic adaptation, an aftereffect occurs at the adapted retinal location after an eye movement, whereas in spatiotopic adaptation, an aftereffect occurs at the adapted spatiotopic location though the retinal location is altered after the saccade. 
The input to the visual cortex from the natural visual world comprises complex stimulus attributes which potentially excite interactions of different cortical levels (Betsch, Einhäuser, Körding, & König, 2004; Bex, Dakin, & Mareschal, 2005; Bex, Mareschal, & Dakin, 2007; Bex, Solomon, & Dakin, 2009; Billock, de Guzman, & Kelso, 2001; Dong & Atick, 1995). Although controlled studies with artificial stimuli elucidate feature-selective neural responses, they might not fully divulge how these mechanisms respond in a coordinated manner with other neural populations when stimulated by the rich natural-image content (David, Vinje, & Gallant, 2004; Felsen & Dan, 2005; Ringach, Hawken, & Shapley, 2002). Studying visual plasticity to stimuli that resemble the natural world thus not only mirrors real-life visual circumstances but even asserts potential coordinated responses of different cortical areas. 
Here, we evaluated whether distortion adaptation is transferred across fixations during eye movements in a distorted natural environment. Reference frames of geometrical skew-distortion adaptation were tested with natural-image contents. After a saccade, skew adaptation was partially transferred to a new fixation only at retinotopic and spatiotopic locations. No aftereffect was obtained at a control location which was neither retinotopic nor spatiotopic. Thus, during eye movements in a skewed visual world, retinotopic as well as spatiotopic distortion information is conceivably used to mediate stable perception. 
Materials and methods
Here, we assessed the coordinate frames in which skew-adaptation information was transferred across fixations. The measurements encompassed four conditions: no saccade, retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control. 
Participants
In total, 12 observers participated in the study. All partook in the no-saccade adaptation condition, eight in the spatiotopic condition, and eight in the retinotopic and control conditions (four of whom also participated in the spatiotopic condition). All except one were unaware of the purpose of the study. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. 
Study protocol
The ethics committee at the Medical Faculty of the Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen and the University Hospital approved the study. In adherence to the Declaration of Helsinki, participants' informed written consent was collected prior to their participation in the study. 
Setup
Psychophysical experiments were designed and stimuli were generated in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA) using the PsychToolbox routines (Brainard, 1997) on an Apple computer (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA). The stimuli were displayed on a ViewPixx/3D monitor (VPixx Technologies Inc., Saint-Bruno, Canada) at a resolution of 1,920 × 1,080 pixels and vertical refresh rate of 100 Hz in an otherwise darkened room. A chin and head rest was used to maintain the viewing distance of 60 cm at which the display subtended visual angles of approximately 47° horizontally × 27° vertically. The lateral position of the stimuli was controlled gaze contingently by recording the right eye's position in real time at a sample rate of 1 kHz with the EyeLink 1000 Plus eye tracker (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) and the Eyelink toolbox (Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002). Participants' responses were recorded using the left, right, and space keys of a keyboard. 
Stimuli
Geometrical transformation matrix Display Formula\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\(\def\bupalpha{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C2}}\)\(\def\bupbeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C3}}\)\(\def\bupgamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C4}}\)\(\def\bupdelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C5}}\)\(\def\bupepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C6}}\)\(\def\bupvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DC}}\)\(\def\bupzeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C7}}\)\(\def\bupeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C8}}\)\(\def\buptheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C9}}\)\(\def\bupiota{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CA}}\)\(\def\bupkappa{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CB}}\)\(\def\buplambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CC}}\)\(\def\bupmu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CD}}\)\(\def\bupnu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CE}}\)\(\def\bupxi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CF}}\)\(\def\bupomicron{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D0}}\)\(\def\buppi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D1}}\)\(\def\buprho{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D2}}\)\(\def\bupsigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D4}}\)\(\def\buptau{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D5}}\)\(\def\bupupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D6}}\)\(\def\bupphi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D7}}\)\(\def\bupchi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D8}}\)\(\def\buppsy{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D9}}\)\(\def\bupomega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DA}}\)\(\def\bupvartheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DD}}\)\(\def\bGamma{\bf{\Gamma}}\)\(\def\bDelta{\bf{\Delta}}\)\(\def\bTheta{\bf{\Theta}}\)\(\def\bLambda{\bf{\Lambda}}\)\(\def\bXi{\bf{\Xi}}\)\(\def\bPi{\bf{\Pi}}\)\(\def\bSigma{\bf{\Sigma}}\)\(\def\bUpsilon{\bf{\Upsilon}}\)\(\def\bPhi{\bf{\Phi}}\)\(\def\bPsi{\bf{\Psi}}\)\(\def\bOmega{\bf{\Omega}}\)\(\def\iGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E4}}\)\(\def\iDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E5}}\)\(\def\iTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E9}}\)\(\def\iLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EC}}\)\(\def\iXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EF}}\)\(\def\iPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F1}}\)\(\def\iSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F4}}\)\(\def\iUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F6}}\)\(\def\iPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F7}}\)\(\def\iPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F9}}\)\(\def\iOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6FA}}\)\(\def\biGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D71E}}\)\(\def\biDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D71F}}\)\(\def\biTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D723}}\)\(\def\biLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D726}}\)\(\def\biXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D729}}\)\(\def\biPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D72B}}\)\(\def\biSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D72E}}\)\(\def\biUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D730}}\)\(\def\biPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D731}}\)\(\def\biPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D733}}\)\(\def\biOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D734}}\)\(M\) was used to skew images by a shear angle of Display Formula\(\theta \), wherein the pixel positions x and y of the undistorted image were remapped to distorted positions Display Formula\({x_{\rm{d}}}\) and Display Formula\({y_{\rm{d}}}\):  
\begin{equation}\tag{1}\left( {\matrix{ {{x_{\rm{d}}}} \cr {{y_{\rm{d}}}} \cr } } \right) = M \times \left( {\matrix{ x \cr y \cr } } \right)\end{equation}
 
\begin{equation}\tag{2}M = \left( {\matrix{ \ \ \ 1\ \ \ \ -\tan\theta \cr -\tan\theta\ \ \ \ \ \ 1\ \ \cr } } \right){\rm .}\end{equation}
 
The adapting stimuli were skewed natural-image sequences. Two groups of oppositely skewed adapting stimuli were generated, containing left-skewed (θ = +25°) and right-skewed (θ = −25°) natural-image sequences. Distortion information from sheared boundaries was removed by weighting each image with a Hanning window weighting function of the second order (Harris, 1978). Each image subtended 10° × 10° of visual angle at zero eccentricity. The image content was identical in the positive- and negative-skewed adapting image sequences. These image sequences were rendered at a rate of 20 frames/s. A white cross image on a black background, skewed at different angles, was used as a test stimulus to examine the aftereffect. At 0° skew, the cross subtended 10° × 10° of visual angle when viewed at zero eccentricity. Viewing was monocular with the right eye while the left eye was patched. Illustration of a skew geometrical distortion with examples of adapting and test stimuli are shown in Figure 1
Figure 1
 
(a) Sketch of skew geometrical distortion. (b) Example of an unskewed natural image with its skewed counterparts from the adaptation image sequences. (c) Example of an unskewed test cross stimulus with its skewed counterparts.
Figure 1
 
(a) Sketch of skew geometrical distortion. (b) Example of an unskewed natural image with its skewed counterparts from the adaptation image sequences. (c) Example of an unskewed test cross stimulus with its skewed counterparts.
Procedure
Participants were informed about the procedure and trained on how to adjust a skewed cross until they perceive it to be undistorted. 
In each condition, the baseline was first raised by exposure to a positively skewed image sequence for 8 min. Subsequently, the test stimulus appeared on the screen. The test stimulus was a cross skewed at a random angle between 3° and −3°. The observers' task was to adjust the skewed cross until it was perceived as undistorted, that is, until the two edges appeared perpendicular. The left and right keys of a keyboard were used to increase or decrease the skew angle with a step size of 0.5°, and the space key was used to confirm the perception of the undistorted cross. Then the negatively skewed image sequence was presented for another 8 min, followed by similar adjustment task. Fifteen trials of adjustment were performed in each aftereffect-measurement step. The point of subjective equality (PSE) was used as an aftereffect-measurement parameter. It was defined by the skew angle of the adjusted cross at which the subjective undistorted perpendicular cross was perceived. 
In all conditions, skewed adapting stimuli were presented in the upper right visual field 2° from the fixation dot. In the no-saccade condition, gaze position remained fixed throughout the adaptation-measurement procedure, as presented in Figure 2a. In all the other conditions, aftereffects were tested after a saccade, as shown in Figure 1b. Just after each adaptation and before the appearance of the test stimuli, observers made a saccade to a new fixation marker located 14° to the left of the previous fixation marker. The transferred adaptation to the new fixation was measured at retinotopic, spatiotopic, or control null locations separately (Figure 2b). In the retinotopic condition, the spatial location of the test stimuli was altered, maintaining its retinal location as the adapting stimuli; whereas the spatiotopic condition was assessed by varying the retinal location of the test stimuli while preserving its spatial location as the adapting stimuli. The control measurement was performed at neither a retinal nor a spatial matching location, to make sure that the measured aftereffects were confined to retinotopic and spatiotopic locations. 
Figure 2
 
Scheme of experiment paradigm. (a) No-saccade (NS) condition: Observers' gaze was fixed and the aftereffect was measured at the same location as the adapter (A). (b) Transsaccadic transfer: Observers made a saccade of 14° amplitude after the presentation of the adapter (A). After 500 ms, the aftereffect was tested at the retinotopic (R) or spatiotopic (S) location of the adapter, in the retinotopic and spatiotopic condition, respectively. In the control condition (C), the aftereffect was measured at a novel unadapted location.
Figure 2
 
Scheme of experiment paradigm. (a) No-saccade (NS) condition: Observers' gaze was fixed and the aftereffect was measured at the same location as the adapter (A). (b) Transsaccadic transfer: Observers made a saccade of 14° amplitude after the presentation of the adapter (A). After 500 ms, the aftereffect was tested at the retinotopic (R) or spatiotopic (S) location of the adapter, in the retinotopic and spatiotopic condition, respectively. In the control condition (C), the aftereffect was measured at a novel unadapted location.
Observers' gaze was tracked in real time during the whole measurement procedure. Around every fixation target, a circular boundary of radius 1° was defined. Whenever gaze was positioned out of this circular boundary, the stimuli vanished from the screen gaze contingently. At the selected refresh rate of the screen and the sampling rate of the eye tracker, this was achieved in a delay of less than 40 ms (Saunders & Woods, 2014). This assured exclusive presentation of the stimulus at the intended retinal and spatial position. Thus, in all nonretinotopic conditions, there was no retinal overlap of the adapting and test stimuli. Eye movements within a radius of 1° from the fixation marker were ignored, to account for microscopic saccades during fixation. 
Data analysis
For every observer, the aftereffects measured in the 15 trials after each adaptation were binned into three by averaging Trials 1–5 into Bin 1, Trials 6–10 into Bin 2, and Trials 11–15 into Bin 3. The PSEs of the right skew-adaptation aftereffects in each bin were subtracted from those in the corresponding bin for the left skew-adaptation aftereffects to compute the aftereffect magnitude—the Display Formula\({\rm{\Delta PSE}}\). In the retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control conditions, the transferred adaptation across saccade was quantified by the first-binDisplay Formula\({\rm{\ \Delta PSE}}\) of each condition as a proportion of the first-bin Display Formula\({\rm{\Delta PSE}}\) of the no-saccade adaptation condition. 
The overall data were computed by averaging the magnitudes of the aftereffects and the percentage values measured from all the observers. A t-test was performed on each aftereffect. 
Results
The adaptation aftereffects of all the observers in each condition are presented in Figures 3 and 4. Figure 3 shows the shift in perception of an unskewed cross after alternate exposure to oppositely skewed image sequences measured in the no-saccade condition. During fixation, the adaptation aftereffect measured at the adapted retinal and spatial location showed a significant positive Display Formula\({\rm{\Delta PSE}}\), decaying within the 15 test trials (ptrials1–5 < 0.01, ptrials6–10 < 0.05, ptrials11–15 > 0.05). Thus, after exposure to skewed stimuli, observers perceived a cross that is skewed in the adapting skew direction to be unskewed. 
Figure 3
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects during fixed gaze: Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the no-saccade (NS) condition.
Figure 3
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects during fixed gaze: Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the no-saccade (NS) condition.
Figure 4
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects after a saccade at retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control null locations. (a) Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the spatiotopic, retinotopic, and control adaptation conditions. (b) The overall transfer of adaptation across a saccade: Aftereffect magnitudes as percentage of the no-saccade adaptation aftereffect. Error bars show standard error.
Figure 4
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects after a saccade at retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control null locations. (a) Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the spatiotopic, retinotopic, and control adaptation conditions. (b) The overall transfer of adaptation across a saccade: Aftereffect magnitudes as percentage of the no-saccade adaptation aftereffect. Error bars show standard error.
Since the aftereffect decayed as the trials progressed, transfer of adaptation to a new fixation location was estimated by the magnitude of the aftereffects in the first five trials. The aftereffect magnitude measured in the saccadic conditions is shown in Figure 4a. In the saccade conditions, similar to the no-saccade condition, positive Display Formula\({\rm{\Delta PSE}}\) occurred in the retinotopic (ptrials1–5 < 0.05) and spatiotopic (ptrials1–5 < 0.01) conditions. No significant aftereffect was measured in the control condition at a location that was neither retinotopic nor spatiotopic (ptrials1–5 = 0.81). After a saccade, 91% of the magnitude of adaptation was transferred to the retinotopic position and 82% to the spatiotopic position at a new fixation (p < 0.01 and p < 0.05, respectively). This is shown in Figure 4b
Thus, during eye movements, both spatiotopic and retinotopic mechanisms preserve distortion information acquired at previous gaze positions. 
Discussion
Reference frames of adaptation aftereffects were assessed to reveal the neural mechanisms that compensate for skew distortions in dynamic natural scenes during eye movements. While the gaze was varied between adaptation and test tasks, adaptation significantly transferred to retinotopic and spatiotopic locations but not to a null location—that is, neither retinotopic nor spatiotopic. 
In this study, we elucidated that the visual system transfers retinotopic and spatiotopic skew distortion information across a saccade. Retinotopic and spatiotopic reference frames were decoupled by an eye movement just before the test task. Accordingly, retinotopic and spatiotopic effects were dissociated and measured at different positions. Partially transferred adaptation in a retinotopic as well as a spatiotopic reference frame was shown. In the retinotopic condition, the adaptation aftereffect was measured at an identical retinotopic position after a saccade. In the spatiotopic condition, the aftereffect was measured at a retinally novel but spatially constant location. Plasticity of neurons in higher cortical areas with large receptive-field sizes leads to retinal-location-invariant aftereffects (Afraz & Cavanagh, 2008; Gattass et al., 2005; Leopold, O'Toole, Vetter, & Blanz, 2001; Mather, Pavan, Campana, & Casco, 2008; Meng, Mazzoni, & Qian, 2006; Suzuki, Clifford, & Rhodes, 2005; Van Essen & Anderson, 1995; Zimmer & Kovacs, 2011). Thus, general position invariance was excluded in a control condition. If the measured adaptation aftereffect at the spatiotopic position were a result of general position invariance, an aftereffect would have occurred at a separate location as well (Ezzati, Golzar, & Afraz, 2008; Mathot & Theeuwes, 2013; Zimmermann et al., 2016). Accordingly, in our control condition the aftereffect was measured at a novel retinal location, which was neither retinotopically nor spatiotopically matched. No significant aftereffect was found. This ascertained that the measured significant spatiotopic effect did not originate from plasticity of position-invariant mechanisms. 
Spatiotopic transfer of visual information can be achieved by neurons either remapping the retinotopic organization of their receptive fields during a saccade (Duhamel, Colby, & Goldberg, 1992; Kusunoki & Goldberg, 2003; Nakamura & Colby, 2002) or comprising receptive fields whose coordinate system is in extraretinal reference frames (d'Avossa et al., 2007; Duhamel, Bremmer, BenHamed, & Graf, 1997; Galletti et al., 1993). In the case of receptive-field remapping, saccadic eye movements trigger changes in the receptive-field location of neurons. If a stimulus initially appeared outside the neuron's receptive field and a saccade which will bring the stimulus into the receptive field is intended, the neuron changes its receptive-field location to the stimulus location before saccade execution. It starts firing at the initial gaze position and keeps this sensitivity after the saccade when the stimulus appears in its receptive field. Accordingly, the stimulus's spatial correspondence will be maintained. In purely spatiotopic neurons, on the other hand, receptive-field organization is based on an extraretinal reference frame and codes the visual stimulus with its location in space. Hence, the neurons keep responding to a preferred feature at any gaze position and retain information during eye movements as long as the stimulus is stationary in space. 
Either one or both of the aforementioned spatiotopic mechanisms could account for the reported transfer of spatiotopic skew information across saccades. In a receptive-field remapping mechanism, skew-information-coding neurons should be sensitive to eye-movement-related signals, such as efference copy and proprioceptive information, to remap their receptive fields before the saccade (Andersen et al., 1985; Backus, Banks, van Ee, & Crowell, 1999; Wexler, Panerai, Lamouret, & Droulez, 2001; Wexler & Van Boxtel, 2005; Wolpert & Flanagan, 2001). In the other case, neurons selective to the skew information would have pure spatiotopic receptive fields. This way, the same neurons process the skewed adapting and test stimuli before and after the saccade, respectively, by maintaining spatial correspondence. 
In addition to stimulation of receptive-field remapping, eye-movement signals alter firing patterns of neurons in the cortical visual hierarchy (Andersen, 1997; Andersen et al., 1985; DeSouza, Dukelow, & Vilis, 2002; Trotter & Celebrini, 1999). Gaze-contingent perceptual changes to distortions of a wedged prism have been previously demonstrated (Pick & Hay, 1966). In the present study, the partial transfer of adaptation to a new gaze position might be explained by changes of neural mechanisms whose responses are modulated by the gaze direction (Nishida, Motoyoshi, Andersen, & Shimojo, 2003; Parwaga, Buckley, & Duke, 2016). 
Aftereffects originate from response changes in specific neural populations tuned to the adapting stimuli's attributes (Clifford et al., 2007; Roach, Webb, & McGraw, 2008; Webster, 2011, 2015). Thus, adjustments in neural substrates processing the altered spatiotemporal features of the skewed natural-image content would have led to the measured aftereffects. These include neurons affected by skew effects such as tilt in contour orientations, dimension asymmetry due to oblique magnification, shift in the angle between the contours, and change in the distribution of motion signals in the direction of the skew. 
Although studies argue that visual coding is purely retinotopic (Gardner, Merriam, Movshon, & Heeger, 2008; Golomb & Kanwisher, 2012a), spatiotopic adaptation at different visual areas has repeatedly been demonstrated in classical feature-selective adaptation paradigms (Melcher, 2005, 2007; Zimmermann et al., 2016). Lower level contrast and contour-orientation processing mechanisms in early stages like V1 exhibit fully retinotopic reference frames (Bao & Engel, 2012; Dekel & Sagi, 2015; Dickinson & Badcock, 2013; Wilkinson, Wilson, & Habak, 1998; Zhao, Seriès, Hancock, & Bednar, 2011). Electrophysiological and imaging studies have confirmed behavioral evidence on spatiotopic coding through receptive-field remapping in visual areas such as V2, V3, and V4 (Melcher, 2005; Nakamura & Colby, 2002; Zimmermann et al., 2016). These areas are well suited to encode intermediate shape information, such as orientation, aspect ratio, symmetry, and angle (Boynton & Hegdé, 2004; Gegenfurtner, Kiper, & Levitt, 1997; Hegdé & Van Essen, 2000; Ito & Komatsu, 2004; Laursen & Rasmussen, 1975; Loffler, 2008; Regan & Hamstra, 1992; Suzuki & Cavanagh, 1998; Wilson & Wilkinson, 2002; Zimmermann et al., 2016). Moreover, motion-processing neural mechanisms in MT+ exhibit pure spatiotopic receptive fields (Melcher & Morrone, 2003; Wexler et al., 2001). Their possible interactions with form-processing mechanisms could contribute to the measured spatiotopic aftereffects (Pavan, Marotti, & Mather, 2013). 
The present study extended these findings to a naturalistic stimulation of the visual system—that is, when the visual system is exposed to dynamic natural-image content. Unlike classical feature-selective adaptation with artificial stimuli, such adaptation from a stimulus containing a variety of attributes potentially involves dynamic interactions between multiple levels in the hierarchy of the visual cortex and coordinated responses of several neural populations stimulated during adaptation (Berkley, Debruyn, & Orban, 1994; Dakin, Williams, & Hess, 1999; Pavan et al., 2013; Poirier & Frost, 2005; Smith, Clifford, & Wenderoth, 2001). In line with the aforementioned feature-selective adaptation studies, our results assert spatiotopic as well as retinotopic visual-coding mechanisms during ecological viewing conditions. Furthermore, our methodology provides a tool to address the contribution of these mechanisms in real-life scenarios—for example, adaptation to optically induced distortions. 
Therefore, during a saccade in a distorted natural environment, distortion processing does not start anew at every fixation. Rather, the visual system preserves distortion information acquired at previous fixations. This is specifically efficient in vision with progressive additional lenses, where the distortions are complexly distributed across the visual field. Specifically, during vision accompanying eye movements, spatiotopic mechanisms might facilitate habituation in this application scenario, since they retain the distortion's spatial layout of the spectacles though their retinal position is varied. 
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Tübingen. Funding received from Eberhard-Karls-University Tübingen (ZUK 63) as part of the German Excellence initiative from the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). This work was done in an industry on campus cooperation between the University of Tübingen and Carl Zeiss Vision International GmbH. S. W. Habtegiorgis is scientist at the University Tübingen; K. Rifai and S. Wahl are employed by Carl Zeiss Vision International GmbH and are scientists at the University Tübingen. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Selam W. Habtegiorgis. 
Address: Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany. 
References
Afraz, S.-R., & Cavanagh, P. (2008). Retinotopy of the face aftereffect. Vision Research, 48 (1), 42–54.
Andersen, R. A. (1997). Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 352 (1360), 1421–1428, doi:10.1098/rstb.1997.0128.
Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K., & Siegel, R. M. (1985, Month DD). Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science, 230 (4724), 456–458, doi:10.1126/science.4048942
Backus, B. T., Banks, M. S., van Ee, R., & Crowell, J. A. (1999). Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception. Vision Research, 39 (6), 1143–1170.
Bao, M., & Engel, S. A. (2012). Distinct mechanism for long-term contrast adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109 (15), 5898–5903.
Berkley, M. A., Debruyn, B., & Orban, G. (1994). Illusory, motion, and luminance-defined contours interact in the human visual system. Vision Research, 34 (2), 209–216.
Betsch, B. Y., Einhäuser, W., Körding, K. P., & König, P. (2004). The world from a cat's perspective—Statistics of natural videos. Biological Cybernetics, 90 (1), 41–50.
Bex, P. J., Dakin, S. C., & Mareschal, I. (2005). Critical band masking in optic flow. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 16 (2–3), 261–284.
Bex, P. J., Mareschal, I., & Dakin, S. C. (2007). Contrast gain control in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 7 (11): 12, 1–12, http://doi.org/10.1167/7.11.12. [PubMed] [Article]
Bex, P. J., Solomon, S. G., & Dakin, S. C. (2009). Contrast sensitivity in natural scenes depends on edge as well as spatial frequency structure. Journal of Vision, 9(10):— 1, 1–19, http://doi.org/10.1167/9.10.1. [PubMed] [Article]
Billock, V. A., de Guzman, G. C., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2001). Fractal time and 1/f spectra in dynamic images and human vision. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 148 (1), 136–146.
Boynton, G. M., & Hegdé, J. (2004). Visual cortex: The continuing puzzle of area V2. Current Biology, 14 (13), R523–R524.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.
Burr, D. C., & Morrone, M. C. (2011). Spatiotopic coding and remapping in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1564), 504–515, doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0244.
Clifford, C. W. G., Webster, M. A., Stanley, G. B., Stocker, A. A., Kohn, A., Sharpee, T. O., & Schwartz, O. (2007). Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Research, 47 (25), 3125–3131.
Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34 (4), 613–617.
Crespi, S., Biagi, L., d'Avossa, G., Burr, D. C., Tosetti, M., & Morrone, M. C. (2011). Spatiotopic coding of BOLD signal in human visual cortex depends on spatial attention. PLoS One, 6 (7), e21661, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021661.
Dakin, S. C., Williams, C. B., & Hess, R. F. (1999). The interaction of first- and second-order cues to orientation. Vision Research, 39 (17), 2867–2884.
David, S. V., Vinje, W. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2004). Natural stimulus statistics alter the receptive field structure of v1 neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24 (31), 6991–7006.
d'Avossa, G., Tosetti, M., Crespi, S., Biagi, L., Burr, D. C., & Morrone, M. C. (2007). Spatiotopic selectivity of BOLD responses to visual motion in human area MT. Nature Neuroscience, 10 (2), 249–255.
Dekel, R., & Sagi, D. (2015). Tilt aftereffect due to adaptation to natural stimuli. Vision Research, 117, 91–99.
DeSouza, J. F. X., Dukelow, S. P., & Vilis, T. (2002). Eye position signals modulate early dorsal and ventral visual areas. Cerebral Cortex, 12 (9), 991–997, doi:10.1093/cercor/12.9.991.
Dickinson, J. E., & Badcock, D. R. (2013). On the hierarchical inheritance of aftereffects in the visual system. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 472.
Dong, D. W., & Atick, J. J. (1995). Statistics of natural time-varying images. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 6 (3), 345–358.
Duhamel, J. R., Bremmer, F., BenHamed, S., & Graf, W. (1997). Spatial invariance of visual receptive fields in parietal cortex neurons. Nature, 389 (6653), 845–848, doi:10.1038/39865.
Duhamel, J.-R., Colby, C. L., & Goldberg, M. E. (1992, January 3). The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science, 255 (5040), 90–92.
Ezzati, A., Golzar, A., & Afraz, A. S. R. (2008). Topography of the motion aftereffect with and without eye movements. Journal of Vision, 8 (14): 23, 1–16, http://doi.org/10.1167/8.14.23. [PubMed] [Article]
Fannin, T. E., & Grosvenor, T. (2013). Aniseikonia. In Clinical optics (1st ed., pp. 329–331). Stoneham, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Felsen, G., & Dan, Y. (2005). A natural approach to studying vision. Nature Neuroscience, 8 (12), 1643–1646.
Galletti, C., Battaglini, P. P., & Fattori, P. (1993). Parietal neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordinates. Experimental Brain Research, 96 (2), 221–229.
Gardner, J. L., Merriam, E. P., Movshon, J. A., & Heeger, D. J. (2008). Maps of visual space in human occipital cortex are retinotopic, not spatiotopic. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28 (15), 3988–3999, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5476-07.2008.
Gattass, R., Nascimento-Silva, S., Soares, J. G. M., Lima, B., Jansen, A. K., Diogo, A. C. M.,… Fiorani, M. (2005). Cortical visual areas in monkeys: Location, topography, connections, columns, plasticity and cortical dynamics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 360 (1456), 709–731.
Gegenfurtner, K. R., Kiper, D. C., & Levitt, J. B. (1997). Functional properties of neurons in macaque area V3. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77 (4), 1906–1923.
Golomb, J. D., & Kanwisher, N. (2012a). Higher level visual cortex represents retinotopic, not spatiotopic, object location. Cerebral Cortex, 22 (12), 2794–2810, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr357.
Golomb, J. D., & Kanwisher, N. (2012b). Retinotopic memory is more precise than spatiotopic memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109 (5), 1796–1801, doi:10.1073/pnas.1113168109.
Harris, F. J. (1978). On the use of windows for harmonic analysis with the discrete Fourier transform. Proceedings of the IEEE, 66 (1), 51–83.
Hegdé, J., & Van Essen, D. C. (2000). Selectivity for complex shapes in primate visual area V2. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20 (5), 61–66.
Howe, P. D. L., Pinto, Y., & Horowitz, T. S. (2010). The coordinate systems used in visual tracking. Vision Research, 50 (23), 2375–2380, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.026.
Ito, M., & Komatsu, H. (2004). Representation of angles embedded within contour stimuli in area V2 of macaque monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24 (13), 3313–3324.
Kusunoki, M., & Goldberg, M. E. (2003). The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89 (3), 1519–1527.
Laursen, A. M., & Rasmussen, J. B. (1975). Circle–ellipse discrimination in man and monkey. Vision Research, 15 (2), 173–174.
Leopold, D. A., O'Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4 (1), 89–94.
Loffler, G. (2008). Perception of contours and shapes: Low and intermediate stage mechanisms. Vision Research, 48 (20), 2106–2127.
Mather, G., Pavan, A., Campana, G., & Casco, C. (2008). The motion aftereffect reloaded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (12), 481–487.
Mathot, S., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). A reinvestigation of the reference frame of the tilt-adaptation aftereffect. Scientific Reports, 3, 1152, doi:10.1038/srep01152.
Meister, D. J., & Fisher, S. W. (2008). Progress in the spectacle correction of presbyopia. Part 1: Design and development of progressive lenses. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 91 (3), 240–250.
Melcher, D. (2005). Spatiotopic transfer of visual-form adaptation across saccadic eye movements. Current Biology, 15 (19), 1745–1748.
Melcher, D. (2007). Predictive remapping of visual features precedes saccadic eye movements. Nature Neuroscience, 10 (7), 903–907.
Melcher, D., & Morrone, M. C. (2003). Spatiotopic temporal integration of visual motion across saccadic eye movements. Nature Neuroscience, 6 (8), 877–881.
Meng, X., Mazzoni, P., & Qian, N. (2006). Cross-fixation transfer of motion aftereffects with expansion motion. Vision Research, 46 (21), 3681–3689, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.012.
Nakamura, K., & Colby, C. L. (2002). Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99 (6), 4026–4031.
Nishida, S., Motoyoshi, I., Andersen, R. A., & Shimojo, S. (2003). Gaze modulation of visual aftereffects. Vision Research, 43 (6), 639–649, doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00007-5.
Parwaga, S., Buckley, D., & Duke, P. A. (2016). Tilt representation beyond the retinotopic level. Journal of Vision, 16 (3): 11, 1–13, http://doi.org/10.1167/16.3.11. [PubMed] [Article]
Pavan, A., Marotti, R. B., & Mather, G. (2013). Motion–form interactions beyond the motion integration level: Evidence for interactions between orientation and optic flow signals. Journal of Vision, 13 (6): 16, 1–13, http://doi.org/10.1167/13.6.16. [PubMed] [Article]
Pick, H. L., & Hay, J. C. (1966). Gaze-contingent adaptation to prismatic spectacles. The American Journal of Psychology, (79), 3, 443–450.
Poirier, F. J. A. M., & Frost, B. J. (2005). Global orientation aftereffect in multi-attribute displays: Implications for the binding problem. Vision Research, 45 (4), 497–506.
Regan, D., & Hamstra, S. J. (1992). Shape discrimination and the judgement of perfect symmetry: Dissociation of shape from size. Vision Research, 32 (10), 1845–1864.
Ringach, D. L., Hawken, M. J., & Shapley, R. (2002). Receptive field structure of neurons in monkey primary visual cortex revealed by stimulation with natural image sequences. Journal of Vision, 2 (1): 2, 12–24, http://doi.org/10.1167/2.1.2. [PubMed] [Article]
Roach, N. W., Webb, B. S., & McGraw, P. V. (2008). Adaptation to global structure induces spatially remote distortions of perceived orientation. Journal of Vision, 8 (3): 3, 1–12, http://doi.org/10.1167/8.3.31. [PubMed] [Article]
Saunders, D. R., & Woods, R. L. (2014). Direct measurement of the system latency of gaze-contingent displays. Behavior Research Methods, 46 (2), 439–447, doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0375-5.
Smith, S., Clifford, C. W. G., & Wenderoth, P. (2001). Interaction between first- and second-order orientation channels revealed by the tilt illusion: Psychophysics and computational modelling. Vision Research, 41 (8), 1057–1071.
Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1998). A shape-contrast effect for briefly presented stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24 (5), 1315–1341.
Suzuki, S. (2005). High-level pattern coding revealed by brief shape aftereffects. In C. Clifford & G. Rhodes (Eds.), Fitting the mind to the world: Adaptation and aftereffects in high-level vision, advances in visual cognition series (vol. 2, pp. 135–172). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Trotter, Y., & Celebrini, S. (1999). Gaze direction controls response gain in primary visual-cortex neurons. Nature, 398 (6724), 239–242, doi:10.1038/18444.
van Boxtel, J. J. A., Alais, D., & van Ee, R. (2008). Retinotopic and non-retinotopic stimulus encoding in binocular rivalry and the involvement of feedback. Journal of Vision, 8 (5): 17, 1–10, http://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.17. [PubMed] [Article]
Van Essen, D. C., & Anderson, C. H. (1995). Information processing strategies and pathways in the primate visual system. In Zornetzer, S. F. Davis, J. L. Lau, & C. McKenna T. (Eds.), An introduction to neural and electronic networks (Vol. 2, pp. 45–76). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Webster, M. A. (2011). Adaptation and visual coding. Journal of Vision, 11 (5): 3, 1–23, http://doi.org/10.1167/11.5.3. [PubMed] [Article]
Webster, M. A. (2015). Visual adaptation. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1, 547–567, doi:10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035509.
Wexler, M., Panerai, F., Lamouret, I., & Droulez, J. (2001). Self-motion and the perception of stationary objects. Nature, 409 (6816), 85–88.
Wexler, M., & Van Boxtel, J. J. A. (2005). Depth perception by the active observer. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (9), 431–438.
Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H. R., & Habak, C. (1998). Detection and recognition of radial frequency patterns. Vision Research, 38 (22), 3555–3568.
Wilson, H. R., & Wilkinson, F. (2002). Symmetry perception: A novel approach for biological shapes. Vision Research, 42 (5), 589–597.
Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11 (18), R729–R732, doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8.
Zhao, C., Seriès, P., Hancock, P. J. B., & Bednar, J. A. (2011). Similar neural adaptation mechanisms underlying face gender and tilt aftereffects. Vision Research, 51 (18), 2021–2030.
Zimmer, M., & Kovacs, G. (2011). Position specificity of adaptation-related face aftereffects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1564), 586–595, doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0265.
Zimmermann, E., Morrone, M. C., Fink, G. R., & Burr, D. (2013). Spatiotopic neural representations develop slowly across saccades. Current Biology, 23 (5), R193–R194.
Zimmermann, E., Weidner, R., Abdollahi, R. O., & Fink, G. R. (2016). Spatiotopic adaptation in visual areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36 (37), 9526–9534, doi:10.1523/Jneurosci.0052-16.2016.
Figure 1
 
(a) Sketch of skew geometrical distortion. (b) Example of an unskewed natural image with its skewed counterparts from the adaptation image sequences. (c) Example of an unskewed test cross stimulus with its skewed counterparts.
Figure 1
 
(a) Sketch of skew geometrical distortion. (b) Example of an unskewed natural image with its skewed counterparts from the adaptation image sequences. (c) Example of an unskewed test cross stimulus with its skewed counterparts.
Figure 2
 
Scheme of experiment paradigm. (a) No-saccade (NS) condition: Observers' gaze was fixed and the aftereffect was measured at the same location as the adapter (A). (b) Transsaccadic transfer: Observers made a saccade of 14° amplitude after the presentation of the adapter (A). After 500 ms, the aftereffect was tested at the retinotopic (R) or spatiotopic (S) location of the adapter, in the retinotopic and spatiotopic condition, respectively. In the control condition (C), the aftereffect was measured at a novel unadapted location.
Figure 2
 
Scheme of experiment paradigm. (a) No-saccade (NS) condition: Observers' gaze was fixed and the aftereffect was measured at the same location as the adapter (A). (b) Transsaccadic transfer: Observers made a saccade of 14° amplitude after the presentation of the adapter (A). After 500 ms, the aftereffect was tested at the retinotopic (R) or spatiotopic (S) location of the adapter, in the retinotopic and spatiotopic condition, respectively. In the control condition (C), the aftereffect was measured at a novel unadapted location.
Figure 3
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects during fixed gaze: Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the no-saccade (NS) condition.
Figure 3
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects during fixed gaze: Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the no-saccade (NS) condition.
Figure 4
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects after a saccade at retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control null locations. (a) Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the spatiotopic, retinotopic, and control adaptation conditions. (b) The overall transfer of adaptation across a saccade: Aftereffect magnitudes as percentage of the no-saccade adaptation aftereffect. Error bars show standard error.
Figure 4
 
Skew-adaptation aftereffects after a saccade at retinotopic, spatiotopic, and control null locations. (a) Overall averages of aftereffect magnitude in the spatiotopic, retinotopic, and control adaptation conditions. (b) The overall transfer of adaptation across a saccade: Aftereffect magnitudes as percentage of the no-saccade adaptation aftereffect. Error bars show standard error.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×