Figure 3A shows the mean monocular and dichoptic positional maps of the normal group. Seven subjects performed the task monocularly (right and left eye, both), and a separate group of 20 subjects performed the task dichoptically. Two subjects performed the task in all conditions (shown in
Figure 2). Normal performance in both viewing conditions was accurate and precise, with accuracy within a tenth of a degree of visual angle on average across all points. However, accuracy varied systematically with visual field position.
Figure 3B through
D shows
x error, radial error, and angular error in the left and right visual field. These figures confirm the presence of clear location-specific biases on all measures. Our focus was on
x error. Positive
x error at all points represents a rightward translation of responses, and negative error a leftward translation. Positive error at all points in the left visual field combined with negative error in the right visual field represents a contraction of responses toward the center, and the reverse indicates an expansion of responses. The contraction might also occur separately within each visual field, manifesting as an eccentricity-dependent pattern of peripheral contraction combined with central expansion, or the reverse.
Figure 3B shows that in the monocular conditions
x error was positive (rightward) on the horizontal axes in the peripheral left visual field, and less pronounced at other positions. This pattern was confirmed by a significant Eccentricity × Visual Field × Axis interaction,
F(3, 150) = 9.24,
p < 0.0001, in an analysis that included both eyes for each subject. The error overall appeared larger in the right eye than the left eye, but this difference did not achieve significance: Main effect of Eye,
F(1, 6) = 3.67,
p = 0.10. Eye did not interact significantly with the other variables (
p > 0.05 for all interactions). Therefore, the monocular data were averaged over the left and right eyes. Further analyses showed that
x error varied with visual field position only on the horizontal axes, confirmed by a significant Eccentricity × Visual Field interaction on the horizontal axes,
F(3, 18) = 7.70,
p = 0.001, but not the vertical axes,
F(3, 18) = 0.19,
p = 0.91. On the horizontal axes, there was a significant effect of Eccentricity in the left visual field,
F(3, 18) = 10.64,
p = 0.0003, but not the right visual field,
F(3, 18) = 0.95,
p = 0.44, confirming that this variation was confined to the left visual field. Tests of pairwise differences showed that in the left visual field, mean error at 7° eccentricity (0.54°) differed significantly from error at all other eccentricities (mean error at 1°, 3°, 5°: 0.01°, 0.13°, 0.25°); the other pairwise differences were not significant. Positively signed error indicates a contraction (alternately compression), of responses in the left horizontal visual field of both eyes of normal subjects.
A similar pattern was observed with the dichoptic data, with larger-than-average positive x error in the left periphery. The Eccentricity × Axis × Visual Field interaction was significant, F(3, 190) = 31.87, p < 0.0001, as was the Eccentricity × Visual Field interaction on the horizontal axes, F(3, 57) = 32.68, p < 0.0001, and in this case, the vertical axes, F(3, 57) = 4.45, p = 0.007. On the horizontal axes, there was a significant effect of eccentricity in the left visual field, F(3, 57) = 45.05, p < 0.00001, but not the right visual field, F(3, 57) = 3.32, p = 0.09. All pairwise eccentricity differences in error were significant in the left visual field (1°, 3°, 5°, 7° mean error: −0.01°, 0.11°, 0.38°, 0.69°). On the vertical axes, the effect of eccentricity was not significant in the left visual field, F(3, 57) = 0.734, p = 0.54, but it was significant in the right visual field, F(3, 57) = 5.54, p = 0.002. Mean error at 1° differed from all other eccentricities; none of the other pairwise differences were significant (1°, 3°, 5°, 7° mean error: 0.15°, 0.23°, 0.29°, 0.30°). These results suggest an expansion of responses on the vertical axes in the right visual field, in addition to compression of responses on the horizontal axes in the left visual field.
Figure 3C shows radial error at each position, using the same convention as in
Figure 3B. There was a negative radial bias in the left visual field indicating radial compression on both the horizontal and vertical axes, consistent with the
x error shown in
Figure 3B. The radial compression on the vertical axes was accounted for by variation in
y error, not shown. (
Y error varied on the vertical axes between the upper and lower visual fields, unlike the left-right variation of
x error).
Figure 3D shows angular error across polar angle for the four eccentricities. The sign of the angular error alternated between positive and negative across polar angles, in a direction consistent with repulsion from the cardinal orientations and attraction toward the diagonals (see also
Figure 3A). Hence, the variation in the response
x position was affected by, and reflected in, radial and angular biases that depended strongly on visual field position.
Overall, normal x error followed a pattern of undershooting in the left horizontal visual field, consistent with a compression of responses in this region, both in monocular and dichoptic viewing. Radial error also showed left compression, but this compression was not confined to the horizontal axes.