It has been suggested that performance in PM tasks is influenced by imposed oculomotor strategies (Bennett, Baurès, Hecht, & Benguigui,
2010a; Makin & Poliakoff,
2011), characteristics of target motion such as velocity (Sokolov & Pavlova
2003; Baurès, Oberfeld, & Hecht,
2010; Baurès & Hecht,
2011; Bennett et al.,
2010a; Zago, Iosa, Maffei, & Lacquaniti,
2010; Baurès & Hecht,
2011; Nakamoto, Mori, Ikudome, Unenaka, & Imanaka,
2015), and the duration of target occlusion before it strikes the line (Peterken, Brown, & Bowman,
1991; Baurès et al.,
2010). Other factors known to influence performance during PM tasks include the duration of visible motion (Sokolov & Pavlova,
2003), the target size (Sokolov & Pavlova
2003; Battaglini, Campana, & Casco,
2013), the presence of background texture (De Lucia, Tresilian, & Meyer,
2000; Battaglini, Campana, Camilleri, & Casco,
2014), motion aftereffects (Gilden, Blake, & Hurst,
1995; Battaglini et al.,
2014), stimulus-to-background contrast (Battaglini et al.,
2013), and the presence of visual distractors (Lyon & Waag,
1995). In addition, prior experience of fast-interceptive tasks has been shown to influence PM performance. For example, expert baseball players mislocate suddenly disappearing targets (traversing left to right on a computer screen) as significantly further ahead than novice players (Nakamoto et al.,
2015). It was suggested that this overestimation was the result of the experts' enhanced capability for motion prediction and that such domain-specific expertise may be advantageous in compensating for neural transmission and processing delays as well as for transient loss of visual information (e.g., from saccades, blinks, or target occlusion).