Open Access
Article  |   March 2018
Focusing and orienting spatial attention differently modulate crowding in central and peripheral vision
Author Affiliations
  • Andrea Albonico
    Human Vision and Eye Movement Laboratory, Departments of Medicine (Neurology), Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
    NeuroMI - Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano, Italy
    [email protected]
  • Marialuisa Martelli
    Psychology Department, Università La Sapienza, Roma, Italy
    Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
    [email protected]
  • Emanuela Bricolo
    NeuroMI - Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano, Italy
    Psychology Department, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
    COMiB - Optics and Optometry Research Center, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
    [email protected]
  • Eleonora Frasson
    Psychology Department, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
    [email protected]
  • Roberta Daini
    NeuroMI - Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano, Italy
    Psychology Department, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
    COMiB - Optics and Optometry Research Center, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
    [email protected]
Journal of Vision March 2018, Vol.18, 4. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/18.3.4
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Andrea Albonico, Marialuisa Martelli, Emanuela Bricolo, Eleonora Frasson, Roberta Daini; Focusing and orienting spatial attention differently modulate crowding in central and peripheral vision. Journal of Vision 2018;18(3):4. https://doi.org/10.1167/18.3.4.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

The allocation of attentional resources to a particular location or object in space involves two distinct processes: an orienting process and a focusing process. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that performance of different visual tasks can be improved when a cue, such as a dot, anticipates the position of the target (orienting), or when its dimensions (as in the case of a small square) inform about the size of the attentional window (focusing). Here, we examine the role of these two components of visuo-spatial attention (orienting and focusing) in modulating crowding in peripheral (Experiment 1 and Experiment 3a) and foveal (Experiment 2 and Experiment 3b) vision. The task required to discriminate the orientation of a target letter “T,” close to acuity threshold, presented with left and right “H” flankers, as a function of target-flanker distance. Three cue types have been used: a red dot, a small square, and a big square. In peripheral vision (Experiment 1 and Experiment 3a), we found a significant improvement with the red dot and no advantage when a small square was used as a cue. In central vision (Experiment 2 and Experiment 3b), only the small square significantly improved participants' performance, reducing the critical distance needed to recover target identification. Taken together, the results indicate a behavioral dissociation of orienting and focusing attention in their capability of modulating crowding. In particular, we confirmed that orientation of attention can modulate crowding in visual periphery, while we found that focal attention can modulate foveal crowding.

Introduction
Crowding refers to the impaired recognition or identification of a target when surrounded by other irrelevant stimuli (Stuart & Burian, 1962; Flom, Weymouth, & Kahneman, 1963a; Flom, Heath, & Takahaski, 1963b; see also Strasburger, Rentschler, & Jüttner, 2011, for a review). It has been reported to occur in a wide variety of tasks, such as letter recognition (e.g., Bouma, 1970; Toet & Levi, 1992), orientation discrimination (e.g., Andriessen & Bouma, 1976), and face recognition (e.g., Martelli, Majaj, & Pelli, 2005). Different models have been proposed in order to explain this phenomenon (see Tyler & Likova, 2007 for a perspective on crowding models), but according to one of the most accepted accounts, crowding can be explained in terms of the erroneous inclusion of features to be integrated within a spatial window, known as integration field (Wolford, 1975; Pelli, Palomares, & Majaj, 2004). Typically, when we identify a target, we have to combine information from several features within the integration field. Crowding occurs when the visual system operates over an improperly large region of the visual field which includes both target and flanker features. This results in an excessive feature integration that prevents target identification (e.g., Pelli et al., 2004; Solomon & Morgan, 2001). Crowding is a phenomenon described in detail, for which we have well-established diagnostic criteria (Pelli et al., 2004), and attempts have been made to account for the computation that occurs within the integration fields (Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan, 2001; Nandy & Tjan, 2007; Balas, Nakano, & Rosenholtz, 2009; Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011; Freeman, Chakravarthi, & Pelli, 2012). According to the integration field account, the size of the integration fields can be measured by calculating the minimum target to flanker spacing needed to achieve target recognition, i.e., critical spacing or distance (e.g., Toet & Levi, 1992). The critical distance between the target and flanker scales with eccentricity, independently of target and flanker size, and the scaling is roughly proportional to half the target viewing eccentricity, although large individual differences in this proportionality have been reported (Bouma, 1970; Strasburger, Harvey, & Rentschler, 1991; but see also Pelli et al., 2004 for variations across studies). Thus, integration fields are small in the fovea, but larger in the periphery. As a result, the isolation of the target in the fovea is typically an effortless act in usual viewing conditions, whereas in the periphery, most often the integration fields include both the target and the flankers. Identification of objects between flankers, such as letters in words, and of multipart objects, such as faces, show this dependency on eccentricity and not size (Levi, Hariharan, & Klein, 2002; Pelli et al., 2004; Martelli et al., 2005; Rosen, Chakravarthi, & Pelli, 2014). By contrast, detection tasks, as well as one-feature judgements, are immune to crowding (Pelli et al., 2004). 
Many studies that measure the identification of known targets, while presented always at the same retinal location, interpret crowding as the bottleneck of pattern perception (Levi, 2008; Pelli & Tillman, 2008; see also Strasburger, Rentschler, & Jüttner, 2011). Other authors, instead, have measured identification under location uncertainty and suggested that crowding is intimately linked to the construct of attention (He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator, 1996; Scolari, Kohnen, Barton, & Awh, 2007; Chanceaux & Grainger, 2013). Indeed, the attention-resolution account argues that crowding reflects the limitation of the spatial resolution of attention (e.g., He et al., 1996; He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator, 1997; Intriligator & Cavanagh, 2001). As a result, closely grouped targets and distractors can be said to lead to impaired target discrimination because the resolution of attention is insufficient to disambiguate the relevant and irrelevant elements in the scene (Scolari et al., 2007). According to this account, critical distance, and thus the integration field, reflects the finest region that attention can select, which determines the extent of crowding (e.g., He et al., 1996, 1997; Tripathy & Cavanagh, 2002; Chakravarthi & Cavanagh, 2007; Fang & He, 2008). Targets that are spaced more finely than the attention resolution capacity cannot be selected individually for further processing and recognition. Attention resolution is often substantially coarser than the smallest details resolvable by vision, i.e., acuity (Intriligator & Cavanagh, 2001). Clearly, the only distinction between the perceptual account and the attentional account of crowding consists in their different predictions on the role of bottom-up isolation and top-down selection processes. 
Several studies have demonstrated that allocating spatial attention to a particular target location or object improves performance in several tasks (e.g., Eriksen & Rohrbaugh, 1970; Eriksen & Hoffman, 1972; Posner, 1980; Jonides, 1981; Müller & Rabbitt, 1989; Nakayama & Mackeben, 1989; Lu & Dosher, 1998; Yeshurun & Carrasco, 1999), by facilitating the discrimination of visual information or enhancing visual information processing (e.g., Desimone & Duncan, 1995; He et al., 1996, 1997). This effect is observable when the target signal is presented alone, but it is particularly pronounced when the identification of this signal is made more difficult by the presence of irrelevant distractors (e.g., Shiu & Pashler, 1994; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Dosher & Lu, 2000). However, most of these studies, rather than a change of the critical distance, measured a threshold elevation effect and therefore did not disentangle low-level masking from midlevel crowding (Lu et al., 1998; Müller & Rabbitt, 1989; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Dosher & Lu, 2000). 
Some studies directly assess the spatial extent of crowding, demonstrating an attentional influence on crowding or an attentional modulation of the critical distance (e.g., Wolford & Chambers, 1983; Van der Lubbe & Keuss, 2001; Huckauf & Heller, 2002; Felisberti & Zanker, 2005; Montaser-Kouhsari & Rajimehr, 2005; Strasburger, 2005; Põder, 2007; Yeshurun & Rashal, 2010; Petrov & Meleshkevich, 2011; Strasburger & Malania, 2013; but see also Scolari et al., 2007). In particular, valid cues decrease the critical distance whereas invalid cues increase it (Freeman & Pelli, 2007; Yeshurun & Rashal 2010; Grubb et al., 2013). These results suggest that under location uncertainty, observers are unable to use the smallest integration field available at a given eccentric location, and then correctly discriminate the target. Thus, attention may enable the selection of the smallest integration field available or enhance spatial resolution by overrepresenting the space in the attended area while simultaneously suppressing the space outside that area (Suzuki & Cavanagh, 1997; Intriligator & Cavanagh, 2001). 
A further controversy in the literature concerns the difference between peripheral and foveal crowding. Most studies on this topic investigate the crowding phenomenon, and how it can be modulated by attention, while focusing only on the periphery of the visual field. As mentioned before, crowding is more pronounced in peripheral vision (Fine, 2004; Pelli et al., 2004), whereas the critical distances in fovea are typically small, covering only a few minutes of arc, close to the acuity threshold (Latham & Whitaker, 1996; Siderov, Waugh, & Bedell, 2013; Coates & Levi, 2014). Thus, given the limited extent of the integration fields in central vision, both crowding and attentional effects are less pronounced and harder to detect compared to those in peripheral locations. Accordingly, some studies reported that foveal crowding is absent (e.g., Strasburger, Harvey, & Rentschler, 1991; Hess, Dakin, & Kapoor, 2000; Liu & Arditi, 2000). Others instead have argued that foveal crowding can be hardly disentangled from overlap masking (e.g., Levi, Klein, & Hariharan, 2002; Levi, 2008; Pelli & Tillman, 2008). When measured foveally (e.g., Flom et al., 1963a; Westheimer & Hauske, 1975; Westheimer, Shimamura, & McKee, 1976; Butler & Westheimer, 1978; Levi, Klein, & Aitsebaomo, 1985; Chung, Levi, & Legge, 2001; Siderov et al., 2013; Lev, Yehezkel, & Polat, 2014) the extent of the critical distance varies from 4–6 min of arc (Flom et al., 1963a; Siderov et al., 2013) to 30 min of arc (Chung et al., 2001). The attentional effect seems to be less pronounced in the fovea relative to the periphery (Flom, 1991; Strasburger & Rentschler, 1995; Leat, Li, & Epp, 1999). However, measuring contrast threshold Strasburger (2005) has suggested that the focusing-in of attention becomes less precise as eccentricity increases. Critically, the size of attentional modulation as a function of eccentricity is still a matter of debate. 
Here we conjecture that differences across studies of attentional effect on foveal and peripheral crowding might also be related to the different processes involved in spatial attention. In particular, the allocation of attentional resources in space involves two distinct processes: an orientation process, which shifts the attentional resources to the relevant location for further processing (Posner, 1980); and a focusing process, which acts as a magnifying lens and allows us to concentrate our resources selectively on a limited space within the environment, while ignoring the rest of it (e.g., Castiello & Umiltà, 1990; Chun, Golomb, & Turk-Browne, 2011). 
Previous studies (e.g., Castiello & Umiltà, 1990; Stoffer, 1991; Benso, Turatto, Mascetti, & Umiltà, 1998; Turatto et al., 2000) suggest that focusing and orienting are distinct and independent processes, characterized by independent mechanisms: One is automatic and exogenous, for short intervals, and the other is voluntary and endogenous, for longer intervals (Turatto et al., 2000). Some studies also demonstrate that focusing and orienting may operate differently depending on the visual conditions. Indeed, even if the control of the attentional focus can be deployed both in the center and in the periphery of the visual field (Henderson, 1991; Benso et al., 1998), peripheral focusing will typically arise at longer SOAs. It seems that in visual periphery orienting might be faster than focusing (Castiello & Umiltà, 1990), supporting the hypothesis of a low efficiency of focusing when it is deployed outside the fovea (Eriksen & St. James, 1986). Our previous results (Albonico, Malaspina, Bricolo, Martelli, & Daini, 2016) have found a behavioral dissociation between the orienting and focusing components of attention in central and peripheral view. In particular, by using single-letter detection and discrimination tasks with fixed target location (no side uncertainty), we demonstrated that although, a strong effect of the orientation component was evident in the periphery, this was not true for the focal component. Vice versa, in central vision, the effect of the focal component was clear. Therefore, it could be that previous studies have failed to find attentional effects on peripheral crowding, and especially on foveal crowding, because they did not take into account the specific role of focusing and orienting. In order to directly compare the effects of orienting and focusing attention at different retinal location the target position uncertainty should be minimized or equated. For this reason, we used a paradigm based on our previous study (Albonico et al., 2016), with no side uncertainty (i.e., the peripheral target always appears at the same location) and we investigated whether focusing and orienting differently modulate foveal and peripheral crowding. In particular, we hypothesized that orienting would have a strong role in the periphery, while focusing would be more effective in reducing the critical distance only at central locations. 
Experiment 1: Peripheral crowding
Yeshurun and Rashal (2010) have shown a clear attentional modulation of the crowding distance under location uncertainty, with a marked reduction obtained in the case of a valid eccentric cue relative to invalid or neutral cue conditions. Here we aim at replicating the attentional advantage while reducing the location-side uncertainty. Based on recent findings (Albonico et al., 2016), we predict the orienting component to dominate the peripheral attentional advantage. In this vein, we used different cue types to elicit the orienting and focusing attentional components: (a) similar to Yeshurun and Rashal (2010), a red dot, as the optimal cue for the orientation component (e.g., Posner, 1980; Castiello & Umiltà, 1990); (b) a small square just the size of the target stimulus as the optimal cue for the focal component (e.g., Maringelli & Umiltà, 1998; Turatto et al., 2000); and (c) a big square large enough to include the whole triplet in the largest spacing condition as a nonoptimal cue for the focal and orienting components (Maringelli & Umiltà, 1998; Turatto et al., 2000). In particular, the dot, acted as the optimal cue for the orienting processing because it directs and anchors attention to the precise target location, but does not convey any useful information about the target extension; indeed the dot, by attracting the attention on an area much smaller than the area covered by the target stimulus, does not induce a proper recalibration of the attentional window size. On the other hand, the small square encloses the target stimulus, and conveys information about the optimal field of integration to isolate the target from the distractors. The small square also informs about the position of the target stimulus, but does not allow the anchoring of attention to a specific point; rather it induces a recalibration of the attentional window to match target extension. For these reasons, we believe that the small square acted as an optimal cue for the focusing process. Lastly, a baseline condition in which the target appearance was not precued was also included. The presentation time of the cue was chosen on the basis of a previous study (Albonico et al., 2016) to enhance the attentional effects. 
Materials and methods
Participants
Sixteen healthy volunteers (12 females, four male; mean age = 22.8 ± 2.0 SD, range = 19– 26; 15 right-handed and one left-handed) participated in Experiment 1. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and none of them reported neurological, psychiatric, or other relevant medical problems. 
All participants were unaware of the purpose of the experiment, and participation allowed the acquisition of course credits. Each participant was asked to sign an informed consent prior to being enrolled in the study, which was carried out according to the guidelines of the Ethical Committee of the University of Milano-Bicocca, and in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure
The stimuli and procedure were modelled on those of Toet and Levi (1992) and Albonico et al., (2016; see Figure 1). Participants were seated in front of a computer monitor (27 in., 600 × 340 mm) with a resolution of 1,920 × 1,080 pixels and a refresh rate of 120 Hz. A chin and forehead rest stabilized their head position and kept the viewing distance constant at 60 cm. Manual responses were collected by using a computer keyboard. The dominant hand was always used. Participants' eye movements were monitored by an SR Research EyeLink 1000 Eye-tracker controlled by SR Research Experiment Builder software (SR Research Ltd, Canada). Although viewing was binocular, only the right eye was tracked at a rate of 1,000 Hz. The Experiment Builder software was also used to control the presentation of the stimuli. 
Figure 1
 
Example of a trial procedure in Experiment 1 (panel A) and Experiment 2 (panel B). The size of both the stimuli and the cues shown in the figure are not the actual sizes used in the experiment, but are adapted to better show the experimental procedure. Experiment 1 was run at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 2 was run in foveal vision. Panel C shows an example of the stimuli used in periphery: Fixation point was shown at the center of the screen, while the target stimulus (here an upright T) was shown on gray background at 10° eccentricity e in the right visual field. Target stimulus was either shown singly or surrounded by a flanker left and right at distance d, measured from the center of the characters.
Figure 1
 
Example of a trial procedure in Experiment 1 (panel A) and Experiment 2 (panel B). The size of both the stimuli and the cues shown in the figure are not the actual sizes used in the experiment, but are adapted to better show the experimental procedure. Experiment 1 was run at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 2 was run in foveal vision. Panel C shows an example of the stimuli used in periphery: Fixation point was shown at the center of the screen, while the target stimulus (here an upright T) was shown on gray background at 10° eccentricity e in the right visual field. Target stimulus was either shown singly or surrounded by a flanker left and right at distance d, measured from the center of the characters.
Stimuli were presented at 10° to the right of the fixation point (measured from fixation to the center of the target) along the horizontal meridian. In particular, this peripheral location was chosen because it was already used in previous studies on focusing (e.g., Castiello & Umiltà, 1990; Castiello & Umiltà, 1992; Albonico et al., 2016). Target stimuli consisted of a capital letter “T” (Sloan-like font, black) of 0.7° (height) × 0.7° (width) of visual angle in size that could appear upright or rotated by 180°. Meanwhile, the flanker stimuli consisted both of a capital letter “H” (Sloan-like font, color black) of the same size, which could appear upright or rotated by 90° (the orientation of the two flanker stimuli was independent). The target size was selected so that accuracy in discriminating the orientation of the target presented alone was at least of 90% of correct responses. Indeed, the average performance (expressed as proportion of corrected responses) across observers for discriminating the orientation of an isolated target of the same size (0.7°) presented at the same 10° of eccentricity in the right visual field was 0.96 ± 0.06. All stimuli had a stroke width of 5 pixels (corresponding to 0.14°). The center-to-center distance between the target and the flankers varied as a factor of the target size from 1 to 7.5 times that size (all factors, 1, 1.2, 1.4, 1.7, 2.1, 2.5, 3, 3.6, 4.3, 5.2, 6.2, and 7.5, correspond to a distance of 0.7°, 0.84°, 0.98°, 1.19°, 1.47°, 1.75°, 2.10°, 2.52°, 3.01°, 3.64°, 4.34°, and 5.25°; please note that at a factor of 1 the flankers touched the target stimulus). These distances span Bouma's (1970) prediction on the crowding range at 10° of eccentricity. A further condition in which the target stimulus was presented without flankers was also included. If presented, the cue could either be a red dot presented at the target location (diameter of 0.28°), a small black square (0.84° × 0.84°, thickness of 2 pixels), a big black square (10.5° × 10.5°, thickness of 5 pixels), both squares surrounding the target, or it could be absent in 25% of the trials. 
Each trial began with a fixation cross (0.6° × 0.6°) presented at the center of a gray screen (mean luminance 89.4 cd/m2), followed after 1,000 ms by one of the three possible cues or by the absence of any cue (baseline). The cue remained on the screen for 300 ms (SOA) after which a second blank gray screen appeared for 100 ms (ISI). After that, the triplet of letters was then presented for 100 ms (Figure 1A). Participants were asked to discriminate the orientation of the T target stimulus by pressing the corresponding buttons on the computer keyboard. Following the participant's response, the next trial started. The fixation point remained on the screen for the entire duration of the trial to help participants maintain their fixation. 
The experiment started with a three-point calibration, and the participant's eye movements were monitored to ensure proper fixation during the entire experimental session. In particular, trials in which the fixation was lost or fell outside a square of 1° centered on the fixation point were discarded and subsequently re-presented to participants in random order. Every participant completed 1,040 trials. Cue types and spacing between the target and the flankers were randomised across trials. 
Prior to the main experiment, a practice session composed of 26 trials (equally divided among cue types and randomized relative to the target-flanker spacing) was administered to let the participants familiarize themselves with the task. 
Statistical analyses
The proportion of correct responses was adopted as dependent measure and the critical manipulations were the cue types and the center-to-center distance between the target and the flankers. Accuracy data were first submitted to two-way, repeated measures ANOVA with Cue (big square, small square, dot and absence of cue) and Target-Flankers Distance (of 0.7°, 0.84°, 0.98°, 1.19°, 1.47°, 1.75°, 2.10°, 2.52°, 3.01°, 3.64°, 4.34°, and 5.25°) as within-subject factors. The effect-size in the ANOVA was also measured by computing the eta squared (η2) and significant differences were further explored by Bonferroni posthoc multiple comparisons (and corrected p values are reported). 
The magnitude of crowding effect was also measured for each Cue condition as the log of the ratio between the proportion of correct responses in the smaller target-flanker distance and the condition of flankers' absence. A one-way, repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the log ratios, with Cue (big square, small square, dot, and absence of cue) as within-subject factor. 
To determine the critical distance needed to reach the effect of crowding, accuracy data were then analysed using the Palamedes psychometric toolbox for Matlab (Prins & Kingdom, 2009). The model was chosen based on the foveal data distributions. The data from each observer at each cueing condition were fitted with the Weibull function:  
\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\(\def\bupalpha{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C2}}\)\(\def\bupbeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C3}}\)\(\def\bupgamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C4}}\)\(\def\bupdelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C5}}\)\(\def\bupepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C6}}\)\(\def\bupvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DC}}\)\(\def\bupzeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C7}}\)\(\def\bupeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C8}}\)\(\def\buptheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C9}}\)\(\def\bupiota{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CA}}\)\(\def\bupkappa{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CB}}\)\(\def\buplambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CC}}\)\(\def\bupmu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CD}}\)\(\def\bupnu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CE}}\)\(\def\bupxi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CF}}\)\(\def\bupomicron{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D0}}\)\(\def\buppi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D1}}\)\(\def\buprho{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D2}}\)\(\def\bupsigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D4}}\)\(\def\buptau{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D5}}\)\(\def\bupupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D6}}\)\(\def\bupphi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D7}}\)\(\def\bupchi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D8}}\)\(\def\buppsy{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D9}}\)\(\def\bupomega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DA}}\)\(\def\bupvartheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DD}}\)\(\def\bGamma{\bf{\Gamma}}\)\(\def\bDelta{\bf{\Delta}}\)\(\def\bTheta{\bf{\Theta}}\)\(\def\bLambda{\bf{\Lambda}}\)\(\def\bXi{\bf{\Xi}}\)\(\def\bPi{\bf{\Pi}}\)\(\def\bSigma{\bf{\Sigma}}\)\(\def\bUpsilon{\bf{\Upsilon}}\)\(\def\bPhi{\bf{\Phi}}\)\(\def\bPsi{\bf{\Psi}}\)\(\def\bOmega{\bf{\Omega}}\)\(\def\iGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E4}}\)\(\def\iDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E5}}\)\(\def\iTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E9}}\)\(\def\iLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EC}}\)\(\def\iXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EF}}\)\(\def\iPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F1}}\)\(\def\iSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F4}}\)\(\def\iUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F6}}\)\(\def\iPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F7}}\)\(\def\iPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F9}}\)\(\def\iOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6FA}}\)\(\def\biGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D71E}}\)\(\def\biDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D71F}}\)\(\def\biTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D723}}\)\(\def\biLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D726}}\)\(\def\biXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D729}}\)\(\def\biPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D72B}}\)\(\def\biSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D72E}}\)\(\def\biUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D730}}\)\(\def\biPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D731}}\)\(\def\biPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D733}}\)\(\def\biOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D734}}\)\begin{equation}PC = \gamma + \left( {1 - \gamma - \lambda} \right)\times \left(1 - {e^{ - {{({x \over \alpha })}^\beta }}}\right)\end{equation}
using a maximum-likelihood criterion, estimating for each participant the parameters Display Formula\(\alpha \) (threshold) and Display Formula\(\beta \) (the slope parameter; see Strasburger, 2001 for calculating the slope from β), with the parameter γ set to 0.5 that corresponds to the guessing rate in a 2AFC task as our, and Display Formula\({\it{\lambda }}\), the lapse rate value typically different from zero since human observers are prone to stimulus-independent errors (Wichmann & Hill, 2001), fixed at 0.02 as suggested in the model (Prins & Kingdom, 2009). The starting value of the free parameters was the same for all participants.  
Following Yeshurun and Rashal (2010), an exponential and a clipped-line model were also applied to the data. The equations used were the same used by Yeshurun and Rashal (2010) and in particular we employed the following equation for the exponential fit:  
\begin{equation}PC = a\left(1 - {e^{\left( { - s\left( {d - i} \right)} \right)}}\right)\end{equation}
where Display Formula\(a\) is the asymptote, Display Formula\(s\) is the scaling factor, Display Formula\(d\) is the target–flanker distance, and Display Formula\(i\) is the x intercept. The Levenberg-Marquardt algorithm, based on chi-square values, was used to fit a two-clipped function (two straight lines in log-log coordinates) to each set of data, with the slope of the first line set to zero; the equation employed was  
\begin{equation}PC = \min\left( {{p_{{\rm{ceil}}}},\left( {a{e^{b\sigma }}} \right)} \right)x\end{equation}
where a is the break point at ceiling pceil. The critical distance was defined as ceiling break point.  
While the peripheral data are similarly captured by a Weibull, an exponential and a clipped function (median R2 0.70, 0.68, and 0.77, respectively), foveal data are not (Experiment 2). Median R2 for the Weibull model applied to the foveal data is 0.62 (range 0.15–0.92). The exponential fit poorly captures the foveal data with a median R2 of 0.51 (range 0.04–0.93) and a failure to model 18 distributions. Failures are not systematic for conditions or subjects. The clipped lines model performs better than the exponential (R2 = 0.48; range 0.06–0.92) with no failures, but more poorly than the Weibull. 
Thus, the Weibull model was chosen over the exponential or the clipped-line model based on the foveal data distributions. In particular, the Weibull function was favoured over other sigmoidal functions (i.e., the logistic function or the cumulative normal) because it has the ability to assume the characteristics of many different types of distributions. 
The function was used to compute the critical distance, defined as the target-flanker distance required to achieve 90% accuracy. This criterion level of task performance was chosen in accordance with previous studies investigating the attentional influence on crowding (Yeshurun & Rashal, 2010; Scolari et al., 2007; however please note that whereas we used a similar criterion, different psychometric function models were used in the two studies cited) and because a higher criterion will be more sensitive in detecting attentional effects in foveal vision where critical distances are typically very small. To analyze the critical-distance differences between cueing conditions, a one-way, repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on critical distance was conducted, with Cue (big square, small square, dot, and absence of cue) as the within-subject factor. Planned comparisons were used to further investigate the difference between Cued conditions and the absence of cue, as well as to test the difference between the small square and dot conditions. Effect-sizes were also measured by computing the eta squared (η2). 
Results
Accuracy
The ANOVA on the accuracy data showed a significant effect of the Target-Flankers distance, F(11, 165) = 37.06, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.428, showing increased accuracy with increased target-flankers distance, as widely showed in many previous studies on crowding (e.g., Strasburger et al., 1991; Strasburger, 2005; Pelli et al., 2004). The main effect of Cue on accuracy was also significant, F(3, 45) = 9.48, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.024, and posthoc comparisons highlighted that the overall accuracy of the dot condition (proportion correct: 0.843) was significantly higher than the accuracy of the other three conditions (absence of cue: 0.797, p = 0.009; big square: 0.808, p = 0.007; small square: 0.798, p = 0.001), which, in turn, did not differ from each other (all ps > 0.05). 
The interaction between Cue and the Target-Flankers Distance was not significant, F(33, 495) = 1.20, p = 0.209, η2 = 0.025, showing that the effect of the dot was significant independent of the target-flankers distance (see Figure 2). 
Figure 2
 
Results of Experiment 1. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted. Please note that functions do not reach the lower asymptote because the target-flanker distance cannot be made sufficiently small in this condition, i.e., it cannot exceed the minimum target-flanker distance of 0.7° (indicated as dashed line).
Figure 2
 
Results of Experiment 1. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted. Please note that functions do not reach the lower asymptote because the target-flanker distance cannot be made sufficiently small in this condition, i.e., it cannot exceed the minimum target-flanker distance of 0.7° (indicated as dashed line).
Analysis on the magnitude of the crowding effect revealed a significant effect of the Cue, F(3, 42) = 3.14, p = 0.035, η2 = 0.182, and planned comparison highlighted that the extent of the crowding reduction was significantly larger in the dot condition relative to the no-cue condition, −0.125 and −0.192, respectively, F(1, 14) = 11.66, p = 0.004, d = 0.92; but no difference was found between the dot and the small square cue condition, −0.125 and −0.175, respectively, F(1, 14) = 2.94, p = 0.108, d = 0.74. 
Critical distance
The median explained variance and the goodness of fit of the Weibull function were R2 = 0.70 with a transformed likelihood ratio = 127.4, respectively. Critical distance was extracted as the respective point at 90% correct on the Weibull fit (as explained in Methods) and an ANOVA was carried out on the obtained critical distance values. One participant was removed from further analysis because, since his data did not reach asymptote level, the estimated critical distance was exceptionally large for this participant (results are shown in Figure 3). The main effect of Cue on critical distance was significant, F(3, 42) = 4.73, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.252. Planned comparisons showed that in peripheral vision only the dot significantly decreased the critical distance compared to the absence of a cue, 2.88° and 4.97°, respectively, F(1, 14) = 6.31, p = 0.025, d = 0.93; whereas neither the small square, 3.76°, F(1, 14) = 2.24, p = 0.157, d = 0.53, nor the big square, 4.66°, F(1, 14) = 0.35, p = 0.56, d = 0.12, were effective compared to the condition in which the target stimulus was presented without cue. Furthermore, planned comparisons also highlighted that the critical distance associated with the dot condition was significantly lower than the critical distance associated with the small, 3.76°, F(1, 14) = 5.86, p = 0.03, d = 0.88, and the big square, 4.66 °, F(1, 14) = 13.2, p = 0.003, d = 0.19. 
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) at 10° eccentricity by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) at 10° eccentricity by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05
Comments
The results of Experiment 1 confirm and extend previous findings that peripheral crowding can be reduced by orienting attention to the target even in the case of moderate location uncertainty (at least at 10° of eccentricity). In particular, Experiment 1 suggests that only the orientation component of spatial attention is effective in reducing the critical distance, whereas the focal component does not seem to affect peripheral crowding (at least at 10° of eccentricity). Note that, despite that the cues were of different sizes, visibility did not determine our pattern of results; indeed if this was the case, we would expect the small square cue (which size was 0.84° × 0.84°) to have a stronger effect compared to the dot cue (with a size of 0.28° diameter); by contrast, we found that the dot cue was more effective than the small squares and the other cues in reducing crowding. 
Experiment 2: Foveal crowding
We have shown that triggering the orienting component of attention produces a reduction in the critical distance for peripheral targets even when presented at a known location (at least at 10° of eccentricity). On the other hand, we did not find an effect of focal attention. Given that previous findings (Albonico et al., 2016) indicate a strong advantage of focusing in foveal detection and discrimination tasks of isolated targets, we predict that focusing is active in reducing crowding in central vision. In this vein we might expect the small square to strongly modulate the foveal crowding range. Because central vision does not involve notable location uncertainty, we expected to observe no effect of the dot cue on critical distance. The conditions in Experiment 2 replicate Experiment 1 at a central retinal location. 
Materials and methods
Participants
Sixteen new participants (10 females, six male; mean age = 24.7 ± 3.0 SD, range = 19–31; 14 right-handed and two left-handed) took part in Experiment 2. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and none of them had neurological, psychiatric, or other relevant medical problems. 
All participants were unaware of the purpose of the experiment, and participation allowed the acquisition of course credits. Each participant was asked to sign an informed consent prior to be enrolled in the study, which was carried out according to the guidelines of the Ethical Committee of the University of Milano-Bicocca, and in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure
The apparatus and procedure were the same as in Experiment 1, adapted for foveal presentations (see Figure 1B); in particular, in this experiment the viewing distance was kept constant at 300 cm. Since the foveal crowding range is small, measurements with the letter triplets must be taken close to the acuity threshold (Latham & Whitaker, 1996; Siderov et al., 2013; Coates & Levi, 2014). Prior to the experiment we measured the isolated size of the T threshold for each participant by varying size in a 40-trial run using the improved QUEST staircase procedure, with a threshold criterion of 75% correct responses (Watson & Pelli, 1983). The adaptive QUEST procedure increased or decreased the T size according to the participant's accuracy. Averaged acuity across participants was 0.040° ± 0.008° (2.4 ± 0.48 min of arc). Even though it is known that visual acuity can be affected by the type of font used, the average acuity we obtained is similar to the one reported in previous studies with Sloan font (Jacobs, 1979; Strasburger et al., 2011). 
The target stimuli (Sloan-like font, center black) used in the experimental session were 0.05° × 0.05° in size (3 × 3 min of arc). Orientation discrimination performance averaged (expressed as proportion of corrected responses) across participants obtained with isolated Ts of the selected size was 0.90 ± 0.07. All stimuli had a stroke width of 1 pixel (0.006°). The distance between the target and the flankers (measured center-to-center) varied as a factor of the target size, from 1 to 2 times the size of the target stimulus (all factors, 1, 1.1, 1.2, 1.3, 1.4, 1.5, 1.6, 1.8, and 2, corresponding to a target-to-flanker distance of 0.05°, 0.055°, 0.06°, 0.065°, 0.07°, 0.075°, 0.08°, 0.09°, and 0.1°). A further condition in which the target stimulus was presented without the flankers was also included. The cue could be represented by a red dot (diameter of 0.02°) or a small black square (0.07° × 0.07°, thickness of 0.006°) or a big black square (0.75° × 0.75°, thickness of 0.012°) or by no cue. Both target stimulus and cue were displayed always at the center of the screen on a gray background (mean luminance 89.4 cd/m2). 
Each trial began with a blank gray screen followed after 1,000 ms by one of the three possible cues or by the absence of any cue (baseline). We chose not to display any fixation point to avoid giving an additional or confounding cue to participants. The cue remained on the screen for 100 ms (SOA) after which a second blank gray screen appeared for 100 ms (ISI). After that, the triplet of letters was then presented for 100 ms. 
As in Experiment 1, participants were required to discriminate the orientation of the T target stimulus by pressing one button on the computer keyboard when it was upright and another when it was inverted. Following the participant's response, the next trial started. Every participant completed 800 trials; cue types and spacing between the target and the flankers were randomised across trials. 
Before the main experiment, a practice session composed of 25 trials (equally divided among cue types, and randomly selected for target-flankers spacing) was run to let the participants familiarize themselves with the task and to practice with response modality. 
Statistical analyses
The proportion of correct responses was adopted as dependent measure and the critical manipulations were the cue types and the center-to-center spacing between the target and the flankers. The same statistical analyses as in Experiment 1 were also applied to the data of Experiment 2. As in Experiment 1 the data from each observer at each cueing condition were fitted with the Weibull function, using a maximum-likelihood criterion. 
Results
Accuracy
The first analyses on the accuracy data showed a significant effect of the Target-Flankers Distance, F(8, 120) = 55.26, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.431, demonstrating increased accuracy with increased target-flankers distance. The main effect of Cue was significant, F(3, 45) = 5.80, p < 0.005, η2 = 0.028), and posthoc comparisons highlighted that only the mean accuracy associated with the small square (proportion correct: 0.782) was significantly different from the mean accuracy of the absence of cue (0.731; p = 0.005), whereas neither the big square (0.746; p = 1.00) nor the dot (0.753; p = 0.602) conditions were. 
The interaction between Cue and the Target-Flankers Distance was marginally significant, F(24, 360) = 1.47, p = 0.07, η2 = 0.031, showing that the small square condition was the best condition in almost all target-flankers distances (see Figure 4). 
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 2. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted.
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 2. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted.
The analysis on the magnitude of crowding effect revealed again a significant effect of the Cue, F(3, 45) = 6.68, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.308: Both the small-square (−0.102) and the dot (−0.118) cueing condition showed significantly bigger effect than the no-cue condition (−0.183; F(1, 15) = 19.79, p = 0.001, d = 0.98 and F(1, 15) = 8.62, p = 0.010, d = 0.88; but they did not differ between each other, F(1, 15) = 0.959, p = 0.343, d = 0.23. 
Critical distance
The median explained variance and the goodness of fit of the Weibull function were R2 = 0.62 and transformed likelihood ratio = 81.4, respectively. Critical distances were extracted at 90% correct as explained in the methods to Experiment 1. The subsequent ANOVA on the critical distance data highlighted a significant main effect of Cue, F(3, 45) = 5.09, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.258; see Figure 5). Planned comparisons showed that the critical distance associated with the small square was significantly smaller than the one associated with the absence of cue, 0.097° and 0.126°, respectively, F(1, 15) = 9.39, p = 0.008, d = 0.78; whereas the critical distance of the big square, 0.121°, F(1, 15) = 0.274, p = 0.61, d = 0.11, and of the dot, 0.112°, F(1, 15) = 2.50, p = 0.13, d = 0.37, conditions were not significantly different form the absence of cue. Furthermore, planned comparisons also demonstrated that the small square condition was significantly different from both the dot, F(1, 15) = 12.81, p = 0.003, d = 0.54, and the big square condition, F(1, 15) = 9.40, p = 0.008, d = 0.65. 
Figure 5
 
Results of Experiment 2. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05.
Figure 5
 
Results of Experiment 2. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05.
Comments
Therefore, taken together, the results of Experiment 2 suggest that the critical distance in foveal crowding can be modulated by focusing attention on the target spatial position. 
Experiments 3a and 3b
Results from Experiment 1 and 2 demonstrate that focusing and orienting attention can differently affect crowding in peripheral and foveal vision. However, since we used a limited number of trials per data point, the goodness of fit (R2 and transformed likelihood ratio) of our model in Experiment 1 and 2 were not “ideal.” This is important since our main conclusions are drawn from the critical-distance data, which are parameters estimated based on those fits. For this reason, to further confirm our results, we conducted two more experiments, one in peripheral vision at 10° eccentricity and one in foveal vision, with an increased number of trials per data point. 
Materials and methods
Participants
Six participants (four females, two males; mean age = 23.2 ± 1.5 SD; range = 21–25; two left-handed) took part in Experiments 3a and 3b. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and none of them had neurological, psychiatric, or other relevant medical problems. 
Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure
The apparatus and procedure used for Experiment 3a and 3b were the same as in Experiment 1 and 2. In particular, Experiment 3a was modelled onto Experiment 1, but an additional target-flankers distance condition of 6.5° was added. Similarly, Experiment 3b was modelled onto Experiment 2, but a further target-flankers distance condition of 0.12° was added. 
Every participant completed 2,240 trials in Experiment 3a (at 10° eccentricity) and 1,760 trials in Experiment 3b (foveal presentation). 
Statistical analyses
The proportion of correct responses from each observer at each cueing condition was fitted with the Weibull function, using a maximum-likelihood criterion, and critical distances were extracted at 90%. Since only six subjects were tested, to analyze the critical-distance differences between cueing conditions, a nonparametric Friedman's ANOVA on critical distance was conducted, with Cue (big square, small square, dot, and absence of cue) as the within-subject factor. In the case of a significant effect, Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks Tests were carried out to clarify the effect of the Cue. 
Results
Experiment 3a
Figure 6 shows the individual fit for data in Experiment 3a; subject S006 was excluded because of a technical error during the experiment. First, the increase in the numbers of trials per data point resulted in better fits compared to Experiment 1, since in this case, the Weibull function fitted the data well (median R2 = 0.95; median transformed likelihood ration = 10.22). 
Figure 6
 
Results of Experiment 3a. Individual Weibull fits of the proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field.
Figure 6
 
Results of Experiment 3a. Individual Weibull fits of the proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field.
The Friedman's ANOVA on critical distance data (see also Table 1) revealed a significant effect of the Cue condition, χ2(3) = 9.96, p = 0.019, and subsequent paired Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks Tests showed that the critical distance associated with the dot (2.70°) was significant smaller of the critical distances of the small square (3.53°, p = 0.043), big square (3.86°, p = 0.043), and of the absence of cue (3.68°, p = 0.043). 
Table 1
 
Critical distance (defined as the respective point at 90% correct on the Weibull fit) data for all six subjects that participated in Experiment 3a and 3b. Notes: Experiment 3a was conducted in peripheral vision at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 3b, in foveal vision.
Table 1
 
Critical distance (defined as the respective point at 90% correct on the Weibull fit) data for all six subjects that participated in Experiment 3a and 3b. Notes: Experiment 3a was conducted in peripheral vision at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 3b, in foveal vision.
Experiment 3b
Figure 7 shows the individual fit for the data in foveal vision. Again, as evident from the figure, the increase in the numbers of trials per data point resulted in better fits compared to Experiment 2. Indeed, in this case, the Weibull function fitted the data well (median R2 = 0.96, median transformed likelihood ration = 18.0). 
Figure 7
 
Results of Experiment 3b. Individual Weibull fits of proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets.
Figure 7
 
Results of Experiment 3b. Individual Weibull fits of proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets.
The Friedman's ANOVA on critical distance data (see also Table 1) revealed a significant effect of the Cue condition, χ2(3) = 13.2, p = 0.04, and subsequent paired Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks Tests showed that the critical distance associated with the small square (0.097°) was significant smaller of the critical distances of the dot (0.105°, p = 0.028), big square (0.112°, p = 0.028), and of the absence of cue (0.111°, p = 0.028). Furthermore, the critical distance of the dot condition (0.105°) was significantly reduced compared to the big square (0.112°, p = 0.046) and the absence of cue (0.111°, p = 0.046). 
Comments
Results from Experiments 3a and 3b confirm the evidence obtained in Experiment 1 and 2, showing that focusing and orienting attention can differently modulate the critical distance in foveal and peripheral crowding. In particular, Experiment 3a confirms that only orienting was effective in reducing the critical distance in peripheral vision (at 10° eccentricity). Experiment 3b showed that, even though both focusing and orienting reduced the critical distance in fovea compare to the absence of cue, the reduction associated with focusing was significantly larger than the effect of orienting. 
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the differential role of focusing and orienting attentional processes in the modulation of crowding and the reduction of the critical distance in foveal and peripheral vision. We found that, equating for task difficulty by measuring the critical distance at the same accuracy level for all conditions and observers, crowding was differently modulated by focusing and orienting attention. Indeed, in the periphery (Experiment 1 and Experiment 3a, that were at 10°) only the dot cue (i.e., the optimal cue for the orienting process) was effective in reducing crowding compared to the absent cue condition, while the big square and the small square were not. On the other hand, foveal results (Experiment 2 and Experiment 3b) show a reduction of the critical distance when the small square was used as a cue (i.e., the optimal cue for the focusing process). In Experiment 2 the large square and the dot did not modulate the crowding range relative to the absent cue condition, whereas in Experiment 3b the dot did reduce the critical distance compared to the large square and the absence of cue, but still this effect was smaller compared to the one associated with the small square. 
Bouma's rule states that the critical distance between the target and flanker below which crowding is eliminated is roughly proportional to half the target viewing eccentricity (Bouma, 1970). In the condition of the absence of cue, our data led to a Bouma's factor (calculated as the slope b of the median critical distances in foveal and peripheral vision) of 0.35 (Experiment 1 and 2) and of 0.40 (Experiment 3a and 3b). Precueing the target letter diminished crowding and the critical distances, though. Previous studies have already demonstrated that directing attention to the target location can reduce the critical distance (e.g., Van der Lubbe & Keuss, 2001; Heller, 2002; Felisberti & Zanker, 2005; Huckauf & Strasburger, 2005; Põder, 2007; Yeshurun & Rashal, 2010), and, according to some accounts of crowding, this would suggest that attention could reduce the size of integration fields responsible for crowding. The reduction of the critical distance that we obtained in peripheral vision is similar to the one found in previous studies that have used similar eccentricity (9°), threshold criterion (i.e., 90% accuracy), and attentional cue (a dot); indeed, in our study the dot induced a reduction in the critical distance of 2.09° compared to the absence of cue (Experiment 1) and of 0.98° (Experiment 3a), while Yeshurun and Rashal (2010) in their experiment 2 reported a reduction of 0.77° and Grubb et al. (2013) reported a reduction of approximately 0.5°. Similarly, previous studies showed that in foveal vision for high contrast stimuli, crowding in normal observers extends over distances on the order of about 4–6 min arc (Flom et al., 1963a; Wolford & Chambers, 1984; Danilova & Bondarko, 2007; Siderov et al., 2013). Our results are in the same range, with critical distances of 7.6 (Experiment 2) and 6.7 (Experiment 3b) min of arc without cue. We also showed that critical distances in fovea can be reduced by attention: The small square induced a reduction of 1.8 min of arc (Experiment 2) and of 0.9 min of arc (Experiment 3b). Taken together, our results from Experiment 1, 2, 3a, and 3b show initial evidence that foveal and peripheral crowding are modulated independently by the two components of spatial attention. In agreement with previous evidence (e.g., Castiello & Umiltà, 1990; Turatto et al., 2000; Albonico et al., 2016), our results show that whereas focusing dominates central vision (even though orienting can still affect performance in the fovea even if to a lesser extent than focusing), it does not modulate peripheral performance. Instead, peripheral processing can be enhanced by the orienting process. 
Reducing spatial uncertainty by presenting the target stimulus at the same location might have hidden the effect of the orientation mechanism, particularly in foveal vision, and further studies would be needed to better disentangle the effect of eccentricity from the effect of location uncertainty. However, our results show that spatial attention could diminish crowding even in the absence of spatial uncertainty (side uncertainty). According to previous studies (Maringelli & Umiltà, 1998), equating spatial uncertainty in the fovea and the periphery enables direct comparison of the effects of orienting and focusing attention at a different retinal location. The dot mainly acts as the optimal cue for the orienting process, anchoring attention to the precise target location. On the other hand, the small square primarily induces a recalibration of the attentional window to match target extension. Our results indicate that orienting and focusing are dissociated. Strasburger (2005) found an eccentricity dependent modulation of the focussing component of attention. Similarly, in comparing the effect of a cue surrounding the target as a function of the cue size, Strasburger and Malania (2013) extended previous findings, showing an overall effect of cuing on threshold, independent of cue size, with no effect on source confusion (target and flankers confusability). Our results replicate and extend these findings. They indicate that “focusing-in” is active in the fovea and sluggish in the periphery, where, similar to Strasburger and Malania (2013), the square cue size doesn't matter. Additionally, by comparing the effect of different cues, we found a qualitative difference: Focusing dominates in the fovea, while orienting in the periphery. This dissociation is a direct evidence of the multicomponential nature of attention in enhancing spatial resolution. 
This dissociation could also be helpful in explaining the reported discrepancies in previous studies on the attentional modulation of the foveal crowding range. It has been suggested that foveal crowding could be less sensitive to the effect of attention compared to peripheral crowding (Flom, 1991; Strasburger & Rentschler, 1995; Leat et al., 1999), because of the limitation of visual acuity. However, our data suggest that attention can modulate both foveal and peripheral crowding through the contribution of separate components. In particular, we propose that focusing would act in the fovea, reducing crowding by overrepresenting the space in the attended area and simultaneously suppressing the space outside of that area (Suzuki & Cavanagh, 1997; Intriligator & Cavanagh, 2001). This is consistent with previous studies, which show that attention can enhance the spatial resolution at the attended location by promoting the processing of information over a smaller area (Yeshurun & Carrasco, 1998, 1999, 2000). On the other hand, orienting would still improve peripheral identification by reducing the target residual position uncertainty and enabling the selection of the smallest integration field available at the target location (e.g., Põder, 2006, 2007). Thus, it could be that previous studies have failed to find an attentional effect on foveal crowding because they did not take into account either the independence of these two distinct processes across the visual field or the different conditions in which they operate. Moreover, as a consequence of our results and interpretation, we speculate that the reason why crowding is more pronounced in peripheral vision could be the lack (or reduction) of focal attention. 
It has to be mentioned that only a single eccentric location was tested in the present study; still, after comparing performance in the fovea and at 10° of eccentricity in the right visual field, our results support the notion that focusing and orienting are dissociable processes (e.g., Benso et al., 1998; Turatto et al., 2000; Albonico et al., 2016). However, it is possible that testing graded eccentric locations would lead to graded differences between focusing and orienting: In particular, the role of focusing may gradually decrease as we move away from the fovea, whereas the role of orienting would increase with increasing spatial uncertainty. A similar argument can be made for the cue timing manipulation since only one SOA interval for each viewing condition was used. In particular, some studies have showed that focusing in foveal vision is best revealed under exogenous conditions (e.g., Benso et al., 1998; Maringelli & Umiltà, 1998); thus, one might expect that longer SOAs would reduce the effect of focusing. Overall, despite this consideration, our results reliably demonstrate that orienting and focusing can have dissociable effects on foveal and peripheral crowding. 
Recently, it has been shown that crowding represents a limit for reading in both normal and pathological individuals. Indeed, in normal individuals, the critical distance for crowding is equal to the critical distance for reading (Pelli & Tillman, 2008), and crowding affects the visual span (e.g., Legge et al., 2007), which predicts reading rate (e.g., Pelli et al., 2007). Furthermore, some studies demonstrate that both developmental and acquired dyslexia could be associated with a pathological crowding, which causes an abnormal integration of the letters presented simultaneously (Bouma & Legein, 1977; Atkinson, 1991; Spinelli, De Luca, Judica, & Zoccolotti, 2002; Crutch & Warrington, 2007, 2009; Martelli, Di Filippo, Zoccolotti, & Spinelli, 2009; Martelli, Arduino, & Daini, 2011). Some authors also suggested that the anomalous crowding found in some of these patients could be due to an alteration of attentional mechanisms (Saffran & Coslett, 1996; Mendez, Shapira, & Clark, 2007). This, in combination with our results, strengthens the hypothesis that the smallest range of crowding across the visual field may be modulated by spatial attention, and that the different components of spatial attention play different roles across the visual field. Further studies are needed to explore how much of the abnormal crowding found in diverse populations can be attributed to a sluggish attentional deficit. 
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Yaffa Yeshurun, Hans Strasburger, and the anonymous reviewers for their insightful and valuable comments on the manuscript. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Andrea Albonico. 
Address: University of British Columbia, VGH Eye Care Centre, Vancouver, BC, Canada 
References
Albonico, A., Malaspina, M., Bricolo, E., Martelli, M., & Daini, R. (2016). Temporal dissociation between the focal and orientation components of spatial attention in central and peripheral vision. Acta Psychologica, 171, 85–92.
Andriessen, J. J., & Bouma, H. (1976). Eccentric vision: Adverse interactions between line segments. Vision Research, 16 (1), 71–78.
Atkinson, J. (1991). Review of human visual development: Crowding and dyslexia. Vision and Visual Dyslexia, 13, 44–57.
Balas, B., Nakano, L., & Rosenholtz, R. (2009). A summary-statistic representation in peripheral vision explains visual crowding. Journal of Vision, 9 (12): 13, 1–18, https://doi.org/10.1167/9.12.13. [PubMed] [Article]
Benso, M., Turatto G. G., Mascetti, C., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The time course of attentional focusing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10 (4), 373–388.
Bouma, H. (1970). Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature, 226, 177–178.
Bouma, H., & Legein, C. P. (1977). Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia, 15 (1), 69–80.
Butler, T. W., & Westheimer, G. (1978). Interference with stereoscopic acuity: Spatial, temporal, and disparity tuning. Vision Research, 18 (10), 1387–1392.
Castiello, U., & Umiltà, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of processing. Acta Psychologica, 73 (3), 195–209.
Chakravarthi, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). Temporal properties of the polarity advantage effect in crowding. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 11, 1–13, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.11. [PubMed] [Article]
Chanceaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2013). Constraints on letter-in-string identification in peripheral vision: Effects of number of flankers and deployment of attention. Frontier in Psychology 4 (110), 1–10.
Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101.
Chung, S. T., Levi, D. M., & Legge, G. E. (2001). Spatial-frequency and contrast properties of crowding. Vision Research, 41 (14), 1833–1850.
Coates, D. R., & Levi, D. M. (2014). Contour interaction in foveal vision: A response to Siderov, Waugh, and Bedell (2013). Vision Research, 96, 140–144.
Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2007). The semantic organisation of mass nouns: Evidence from semantic refractory access dysphasia. Cortex, 43 (8), 1057–1067.
Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2009). The relationship between visual crowding and letter confusability: Towards an understanding of dyslexia in posterior cortical atrophy. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26 (5), 471–498.
Danilova, M. V., & Bondarko, V. M. (2007). Foveal contour interactions and crowding effects at the resolution limit of the visual system. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 25, 1–18, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.25. [PubMed] [Article]
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18 (1), 193–222.
Dosher, B. A., & Lu, Z. L. (2000). Mechanisms of perceptual attention in precuing of location. Vision Research, 40 (10), 1269–1292.
Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1972). Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays. Perception and Psychophysics, 12 (2), 201–204.
Eriksen, C. W., & Rohrbaugh, J. W. (1970). Some factors determining efficiency of selective attention. American Journal of Psychology, 83, 330–342.
Eriksen, C. W., & St James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40 (4), 225–240.
Fang, F., & He, S. (2008). Crowding alters the spatial distribution of attention modulation in human primary visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8 (9): 6, 1–9, https://doi.org/10.1167/8.9.6. [PubMed] [Article]
Felisberti, F. M., & Zanker, J. M. (2005). Attention modulates perception of transparent motion. Vision Research, 45 (19), 2587–2599.
Fine, E. M. (2004). The relative benefit of word context is a constant proportion of letter identification time. Perception & Psychophysics, 66 (6), 897–907.
Flom, M. C. (1991). Contour interaction and the crowding effect. Problems in Optometry, 3 (2), 237–257.
Flom, M. C., Heath, G. G., & Takahaski, E. (1963b). Contour interaction and visual resolution: Contralateral effects. Science, 142, 979–980.
Flom, M. C., Weymouth, F. W., & Kahneman, D. (1963a). Visual resolution and contour interaction. JOSA, 53 (9), 1026–1032.
Freeman, J., Chakravarthi, R., & Pelli, D. G. (2012). Substitution and pooling in crowding. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74 (2): 379–396. PMC.
Freeman, J., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). An escape from crowding. Journal of Vision 7 (2): 22, 1–14, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.22. [PubMed] [Article: http://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2122126
Freeman, J., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). Metamers of the ventral stream. Nature Neuroscience, 14 (9), 1195–1201.
Grubb, M.A., Behrmann, M., Egan, R., Minshew, N.J., Heeger, D.J., & Carrasco, M. (2013). Exogenous spatial attention: Evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Vision, 13 (14): 9, 1–13, https://doi.org/10.1167/13.14.9. [PubMed] [Article]
He, S., Cavanagh, P., & Intriligator, J. (1996). Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature, 383 (6598), 334–337.
He, S., Cavanagh, P., & Intriligator, J. (1997). Attentional resolution. Trends in cognitive sciences, 1 (3), 115–121.
Henderson, J. M. (1991). Stimulus discrimination following covert attentional orienting to an exogenous cue. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 91–106.
Hess, R. F., Dakin, S. C., & Kapoor, N. (2000). The foveal “crowding” effect: Physics or physiology? Vision Research, 40 (4), 365–370.
Huckauf, A., & Heller, D. (2002). Spatial selection in peripheral letter recognition: In search of boundary conditions. Acta Psychologica, 111 (1), 101–123.
Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43 (3), 171–216.
Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement. Attention and Performance IX, 9, 187–203.
Latham, K., & Whitaker, D. (1996). Relative roles of resolution and spatial interference in foveal and peripheral vision. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 16 (1), 49–57.
Leat, S. J., Li, W., & Epp, K. (1999). Crowding in central and eccentric vision: The effects of contour interaction and attention. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 40 (2), 504–512.
Legge, G. E., Cheung, S. H., Yu, D., Chung, S. T., Lee, H. W., & Owens, D. P. (2007). The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 9, 1–15, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.9. [PubMed] [Article]
Lev, M., Yehezkel, O., & Polat, U. (2014). Uncovering foveal crowding? Scientific Reports, 4 (4067), 1–6.
Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research, 48 (5), 635–654.
Levi, D. M., Hariharan, S., & Klein, S. A. (2002). Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in peripheral vision: Peripheral crowding is neither size invariant nor simple contrast masking. Journal of Vision, 2 (2): 3, 167–177, https://doi.org/10.1167/2.2.3. [PubMed] [Article]
Levi, D. M., Klein, S. A., & Aitsebaomo, A. P. (1985). Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification. Vision Research, 25 (7), 963–977.
Levi, D. M., Klein, S. A., & Hariharan, S. (2002). Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in foveal vision: Foveal crowding is simple contrast masking. Journal of Vision, 2 (2): 2, 140–166, https://doi.org/10.1167/2.2.2. [PubMed] [Article]
Liu, L., & Arditi, A. (2000). Apparent string shortening concomitant with letter crowding. Vision Research, 40 (9), 1059–1067.
Lu, Z. L., & Dosher, B. A. (1998). External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Research, 38 (9), 1183–1198.
Maringelli, F., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The control of the attentional focus. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10 (3), 225–246.
Martelli, M., Di Filippo, G., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2009). Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision, 9 (4): 14, 1–18, https://doi.org/10.1167/9.4.14. [PubMed] [Article]
Martelli, M., Majaj, N. J., & Pelli, D. G. (2005). Are faces processed like words? A diagnostic test for recognition by parts. Journal of Vision 5 (1): 6, 58–70, https://doi.org/10.1167/5.1.6. [PubMed] [Article]
Mendez, M. F., Shapira, J. S., & Clark, D. G. (2007). “Apperceptive” alexia in posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex, 43 (2), 264–270.
Montaser-Koushari, L., & Rajimehr, R. (2005). Subliminal attentional modulation in crowding condition. Vision Research, 45, 839–844.
Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15 (2), 315–330.
Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vision Research 29: 1631–1647.
Nandy, A. S., & Tjan, B. S. (2007). The nature of letter crowding as revealed by first- and second-order classification images. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 5, 1–26, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.5. [PubMed] [Article]
Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001). Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature Neuroscience, 4 (7), 739–744.
Pelli, D. G., Palomares, M., & Majaj, N. J. (2004). Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distinguishing feature integration from detection. Journal of Vision, 4 (12): 12, 1136–1169, https://doi.org/10.1167/4.12.12. [PubMed] [Article]
Pelli, D. G., & Tillman, K. A. (2008). The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 11 (10), 1129–1135.
Pelli, D. G., Tillman, K. A., Freeman, J., Su, M., Berger, T. D., & Majaj, N. J. (2007). Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 20, 1–36, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.20. [PubMed] [Article]
Petrov, Y., & Meleshkevich, O. (2011). Locus of spatial attention determines inward-outward anisotropy in crowding. Journal of Vision, 11 (4): 1, 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/11.4.1. [PubMed] [Article]
Põder, E. (2006). Crowding, feature integration, and two kinds of “attention.” Journal of Vision 6 (2): 7, 163–169, https://doi.org/10.1167/6.2.7. [PubMed] [Article]
Põder, E. (2007). Effect of center pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions. Psychological Research, 71 (6), 641–645.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32 (1), 3–25.
Prins, N., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2009). Palamedes: Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data. Retrieved from http://www.palamedestoolbox.org
Rosen, S., Chakravarthi, R., & Pelli, D. G. (2014) The Bouma law of crowding, revised: Critical spacing is equal across parts, not objects. Journal of Vision 14 (6): 10, 1–15, https://doi.org/10.1167/14.6.10. [PubMed] [Article]
Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1996). “Attentional dyslexia” in Alzheimer's disease: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 205–228.
Scolari, M., Kohnen, A., Barton, B., & Awh, E. (2007). Spatial attention, preview, and popout: Which factors influence critical spacing in crowded displays? Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 7, 1–23, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.7. [PubMed] [Article]
Shiu, L-P., & Pashler, H. (1994). Spatial attention and Vernier acuity. Vision Research, 35 (3), 337–343.
Siderov, J., Waugh, S. J., & Bedell, H. E. (2013). Foveal contour interaction for low contrast acuity targets. Vision Research, 77, 10–13.
Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. J. (2001). Odd-men-out are poorly localized in brief exposures. Journal of Vision, 1 (1): 2, 9–17, https://doi.org/10.1167/1.1.2. [PubMed] [Article]
Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Judica, A., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Crowding effects on word identification in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 38 (2), 179–200.
Stoffer, T. H. (1991). Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 53 (2), 127–135.
Strasburger, H. (2001). Converting between measures of slope of the psychometric function. Perception & Psychophysics 63 (8), 1348–1355.
Strasburger, H. (2005). Unfocussed spatial attention underlies the crowding effect in indirect form vision. Journal of Vision, 5 (11): 8, 1024–1037, https://doi.org/10.1167/5.11.8. [PubMed] [Article]
Strasburger, H., Harvey, L. O., & Rentschler, I. (1991). Contrast thresholds for identification of numeric characters in direct and eccentric view. Perception & Psychophysics, 49 (6), 495–508.
Strasburger, H., & Malania, M. (2013). Source confusion is a major cause of crowding. Journal of Vision 13 (1): 24, 1–20, https://doi.org/10.1167/13.1.24. [PubMed] [Article]
Strasburger, H., & Rentschler, I. (1995). Is the crowding effect of purely attentional origin? Perception, 24 (Suppl.), 77.
Strasburger, H., Rentschler, I., & Jüttner, M. (2011). Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: A review. Journal of Vision, 11 (5): 13, 1–82, https://doi.org/10.1167/11.5.13. [PubMed] [Article]
Stuart, J. A., & Burian, H. M. (1962). A study of separation difficulty: Its relationship to visual acuity in normal and amblyopic eyes. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 53 (3), 471–477.
Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: An attentional repulsion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23 (2), 443–463.
Toet, A., & Levi, D. M. (1992). The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vision Research, 32 (7), 1349–1357.
Tripathy, S. P., & Cavanagh, P. (2002). The extent of crowding in peripheral vision does not scale with target size. Vision Research, 42 (20), 2357–2369.
Turatto, M., Benso, F., Facoetti, A., Galfano, G., Mascetti, G. G., & Umiltà, C. (2000). Automatic and voluntary focusing of attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 62 (5), 935–952.
Tyler, C. W., & Likova, L. T. (2007). Crowding: A neuroanalytic approach. Journal of Vision, 7 (2): 16, 1–9, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.16. [PubMed] [Article]
Van der Lubbe, R. H., & Keuss, P. J. (2001). Focused attention reduces the effect of lateral interference in multi-element arrays. Psychological Research, 65 (2), 107–118.
Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophysics, 33, 113–120.
Westheimer, G., & Hauske, G. (1975). Temporal and spatial interference with vernier acuity. Vision Research, 15 (10), 1137–1141.
Westheimer, G., Shimamura, K., & McKee, S. P. (1976). Interference with line-orientation sensitivity. JOSA, 66 (4), 332–338.
Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 63 (8), 1293–1313.
Wolford, G. (1975). Perturbation model for letter identification. Psychological Review, 82, 184–199.
Wolford, G., & Chambers, L. (1983). Lateral masking as a function of spacing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 33 (2), 129–138.
Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (1998). Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature, 396 (6706), 72–75.
Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (1999). Spatial attention improves performance in spatial resolution tasks. Vision Research, 39 (2), 293–306.
Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (2000). The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation. Nature Neuroscience, 3 (6), 622–627.
Yeshurun, Y., & Rashal, E. (2010). Precueing attention to the target location diminishes crowding and reduces the critical distance. Journal of Vision, 10 (10): 16, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1167/10.10.16. [PubMed] [Article]
Figure 1
 
Example of a trial procedure in Experiment 1 (panel A) and Experiment 2 (panel B). The size of both the stimuli and the cues shown in the figure are not the actual sizes used in the experiment, but are adapted to better show the experimental procedure. Experiment 1 was run at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 2 was run in foveal vision. Panel C shows an example of the stimuli used in periphery: Fixation point was shown at the center of the screen, while the target stimulus (here an upright T) was shown on gray background at 10° eccentricity e in the right visual field. Target stimulus was either shown singly or surrounded by a flanker left and right at distance d, measured from the center of the characters.
Figure 1
 
Example of a trial procedure in Experiment 1 (panel A) and Experiment 2 (panel B). The size of both the stimuli and the cues shown in the figure are not the actual sizes used in the experiment, but are adapted to better show the experimental procedure. Experiment 1 was run at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 2 was run in foveal vision. Panel C shows an example of the stimuli used in periphery: Fixation point was shown at the center of the screen, while the target stimulus (here an upright T) was shown on gray background at 10° eccentricity e in the right visual field. Target stimulus was either shown singly or surrounded by a flanker left and right at distance d, measured from the center of the characters.
Figure 2
 
Results of Experiment 1. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted. Please note that functions do not reach the lower asymptote because the target-flanker distance cannot be made sufficiently small in this condition, i.e., it cannot exceed the minimum target-flanker distance of 0.7° (indicated as dashed line).
Figure 2
 
Results of Experiment 1. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted. Please note that functions do not reach the lower asymptote because the target-flanker distance cannot be made sufficiently small in this condition, i.e., it cannot exceed the minimum target-flanker distance of 0.7° (indicated as dashed line).
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) at 10° eccentricity by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05
Figure 3
 
Results of Experiment 1. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) at 10° eccentricity by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 2. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted.
Figure 4
 
Results of Experiment 2. Proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM) with a Weibull function fitted.
Figure 5
 
Results of Experiment 2. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05.
Figure 5
 
Results of Experiment 2. Critical distance (center-to-center defined as being at 90% correct) by cue type. Error bars = standard error of the mean (SEM); * indicates p < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
Results of Experiment 3a. Individual Weibull fits of the proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field.
Figure 6
 
Results of Experiment 3a. Individual Weibull fits of the proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for 0.7° letter-T targets at 10° in the right visual field.
Figure 7
 
Results of Experiment 3b. Individual Weibull fits of proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets.
Figure 7
 
Results of Experiment 3b. Individual Weibull fits of proportion of correct responses as a function of target-flankers distance and cue type for foveal presentation of 0.05° letter-T targets.
Table 1
 
Critical distance (defined as the respective point at 90% correct on the Weibull fit) data for all six subjects that participated in Experiment 3a and 3b. Notes: Experiment 3a was conducted in peripheral vision at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 3b, in foveal vision.
Table 1
 
Critical distance (defined as the respective point at 90% correct on the Weibull fit) data for all six subjects that participated in Experiment 3a and 3b. Notes: Experiment 3a was conducted in peripheral vision at 10° eccentricity, whereas Experiment 3b, in foveal vision.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×