Classical studies concluded that microsaccades are conjugate movements (Ciuffreda & Tannen,
1995; Krauskopf, Cornsweet, & Riggs,
1960; Schulz,
1984). For example, by using the high-resolution optical lens lever method, Krauskopf et al. (
1960) reported that more than 98% of saccades greater than 1 arcmin possess highly correlated directions and amplitudes in the two eyes. More recent studies, however, reported that monocular microsaccades occur frequently, representing a sizable fraction of the total number of microsaccades, from approximately 12% to 40% (Engbert & Kliegl,
2003a; Gautier, Bedell, Siderov, & Waugh,
2016; Kloke, Jaschinski, & Jainta,
2009; Martinez-Conde, Macknik, Troncoso, & Dyar,
2006; Valsecchi & Gegenfurtner,
2015). These high numbers of monocular microsaccades are surprising not only because they contradict earlier studies, but also in light of the recent observation that microsaccades serve a similar gaze-centering explorative function as larger saccades (Rucci & Poletti,
2015). Using new methods to determine the location of the line of sight in the scene (a vexing problem in oculomotor research), recent studies have shown that in high-acuity tasks microsaccades precisely relocate a preferred retinal locus of fixation according to the ongoing demands of the task (Poletti, Listorti, & Rucci,
2013; Stevenson & Roorda,
2005). Thus, while it is conceivable that specific alignments of stimuli in depth may trigger monocular microsaccades, one would not expect to see them in experiments in which subjects fixate stimuli in front of them, especially on flat displays.