A key unresolved question in the study of eye movements is to what extent they can be considered independent of perceptual experience. Several recent reviews have summarized our knowledge of the relationship between smooth pursuit eye movements and perception (Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner,
2011; Spering & Montagnini,
2011). In some cases, it has been shown that visual perception of a moving target and pursuit eye movements are strongly linked. Direction biases seen in perception (such as the oblique effect) can also be seen in smooth pursuit trajectories (Krukowski & Stone,
2005). In addition, direction discrimination thresholds for smooth pursuit and perception have been shown to be similar and also show high trial-by-trial covariation (Mukherjee, Battifarano, Simoncini, & Osborne,
2015; Stone & Krauzlis,
2003). Recent work has also shown that smooth pursuit eye movements and perception show similar illusory shifts in direction perception when tracking a stimulus with both internal and envelope motion (Lisi & Cavanagh,
2015). These results have therefore been used to argue that smooth pursuit eye movements and direction perception share some neural mechanisms and are not processed entirely separately.
For speed perception, however, the data are more mixed. There are qualitative similarities between smooth pursuit and speed perception: For example, smooth pursuit acceleration (at the beginning of a trial) is reduced for isoluminant color stimuli (Braun et al.,
2008), and it has been shown in many studies that isoluminant color stimuli are perceived to be moving more slowly than luminance stimuli of matched contrast during fixation (Braun et al.,
2008; Cavanagh, Tyler, & Favreau,
1984; Gegenfurtner & Hawken,
1996a; Gegenfurtner & Hawken,
1996b). Similarly, reduced contrast luminance stimuli are both perceived as moving more slowly (Thompson,
1982) and are pursued more slowly (Spering, Kerzel, Braun, Hawken, & Gegenfurtner,
2005). Finally, a number of studies have found good matches between speed discrimination thresholds for perception and pursuit (Gegenfurtner, Xing, Scott, & Hawken,
2003; Kowler & McKee,
1987; Mukherjee et al.,
2015).
However, there have also been findings that suggest a dissociation between speed perception and pursuit. Several studies have found that, when tracking, participants perceive the speed of chromatic stimuli accurately, contrasting with studies that have found reductions in the speed of smooth pursuit (Braun et al.,
2008; Cavanagh et al.,
1984; Terao & Murakami,
2011). Pursuit has been shown to be more accurate than perception in an experiment involving detecting a velocity perturbation within a moving target (Tavassoli & Ringach,
2010), and there appears to be no trial-by-trial covariation between speed perception and pursuit (Braun, Pracejus, & Gegenfurtner,
2006; Gegenfurtner et al.,
2003). The differences seen between speed and direction when considering the relationship between pursuit and perception are probably due in part to the more complex calculations required for speed perception. The direction tuning of individual MT neurons can be directly related to the direction of pursuit; however, MT neurons respond to a range of speeds, and therefore speed has to be calculated from the population response (Schütz et al.,
2011).
Many studies have used relatively simple experimental paradigms, where a small pursuit target moves across a uniform background. However, there is increasing interest in studying more complex tasks. In one experiment (Spering & Gegenfurtner,
2007) participants were asked to pursue a target and judge its speed when either the target speed or the speed of a peripheral grating surrounding the path of the target could be perturbed. In this case, pursuit and perception were found to be very different, with pursuit showing an integration of the target and context speed, but perception showing a contrast between the two speeds.
Speed judgments can also be made more complex by incorporating both carrier and envelope motion within the target. It is well known that speed and direction perception can be affected by the internal motion of a moving target whilst a participant is fixating at a specific point (Hisakata, Terao, & Murakami,
2013; Hughes, Fawcett, & Tolhurst,
2015; Lisi & Cavanagh,
2015; Shapiro, Lu, Huang, Knight, & Ennis,
2010; Zhang, Yeh, & De Valois,
1993). One recent study has shown quantitatively that speed judgments can also be biased in a similar manner while tracking the target (Hall et al.,
2016). Furthermore, it is known that steady state smooth pursuit to this type of second order stimulus is less precisely matched to the velocity of the target, particularly for relatively slow motion (1°/s), and the resulting errors are corrected by saccades (Butzer, Ilg, & Zanker,
1997; Hawken & Gegenfurtner,
2001). However, perceptual judgments and smooth pursuit eye movements have not previously been studied in the same task, and therefore it is of interest to investigate whether the biases in speed perception map onto smooth pursuit errors in a systematic manner.
Therefore, in this study, we have further investigated the speed perception of Gabor stimuli that contained either a static carrier or a carrier moving in the same or the opposite direction as the overall envelope motion. We first conducted a preliminary experiment where participants made speed perception judgments in a two-interval forced choice task. We predicted that subjects would show speed biases in line with previous results (Hisakata et al.,
2013; Lisi & Cavanagh,
2015; Shapiro et al.,
2010; Zhang et al.,
1993), with perceived speed being slower for carrier motion in the opposite direction to overall envelope motion and faster for carrier motion in the same direction. We then carried out the main experiment, where participants tracked the moving stimulus and were again asked to make a perceptual speed judgment. During the trial, we also measured the speed of the smooth pursuit made to the target. We asked whether the speed of smooth pursuit and the perceptual judgments showed similarities overall and also analyzed whether perception and smooth pursuit showed a correlation on a trial-by-trial basis. We predicted, based on previous results (Braun et al.,
2006; Gegenfurtner et al.,
2003; Kowler & McKee,
1987; Mukherjee et al.,
2015; Tavassoli & Ringach,
2010), that there would be an overall correlation but not a trial-by-trial correlation between smooth pursuit speed and perceptual judgments on this task.
To preview the results, we found that when participants tracked the targets there were strong and consistent differences in smooth pursuit speed between different drift types, but that these differences were smaller and more variable in the case of the simultaneous perceptual judgments. This contrasts with the preliminary data, which showed strong perceptual biases when the participants were fixating, and these biases were qualitatively similar to those seen in smooth pursuit speed. We extended previous work by also considering whether saccadic eye movements differed between the different drift types, and whether this could help us to understand the differences between perception and smooth pursuit in this task. We found that there were significantly different numbers of catch-up (in the direction of target motion) and step-back (opposite to target motion) saccades in the different conditions, and that perceptual judgments significantly correlated with the number of catch-up saccades. We also found that the error in eye position compared to target position differed between different conditions, and that there was a significant correlation between the error immediately prior to a saccade and the perceptual judgments.