Eye movements are usually made spontaneously when observers engage in visually-guided manual tasks such as reaching, grasping, pointing, or hitting. Eye and hand movements are spatially and temporally coordinated: gaze leads the hand by up to 1 s (Ballard, Hayhoe, Li, & Whitehead,
1992; Smeets et al.,
1996; Sailer, Flanagan, & Johansson,
2005; Land,
2006), and gaze locations are anchored to future contact points on the target, indicating strong spatial coupling (van Donkelaar, Lee, & Gellman,
1994; Neggers & Bekkering,
2000; Gribble, Everling, Ford, & Mattar,
2002; Brenner & Smeets,
2011; Cesqui, Mezzetti, Lacquaniti, & d'Avella,
2015; Vazquez, Federici, & Pesaran,
2017). Many of these studies have focused on the saccade-to-reach relationship. Using the same motion prediction task as in the current study, we recently extended these findings to smooth pursuit, revealing a close relationship between the accuracy of pursuit and the accuracy of manual interceptions (Fooken, Yeo et al.,
2016). This link was closest at the time of interception, indicating a common spatiotemporal framework for the control of smooth pursuit and interceptive hand movements. One potential consequence of such common mechanisms would be that improvements in one domain—the eye—should transfer to the other—the hand. Yet, the current study showed that training eye movements alone was not sufficient to improve hand movements, revealing no transfer from eye to hand (
Figure 3). This result was obtained regardless of the type of eye movement training employed (i.e., with or without feedback). Transfer of learning across modalities might only be possible if task requirements are strongly aligned and rely on the same processing mechanisms. Szpiro et al. (
2014) observed transfer from perception to pursuit in a motion discrimination task that required perceptual estimation of the target's motion direction. There is considerable overlap in the neural mechanisms underlying motion perception and smooth pursuit (Lisberger,
2010; Osborne, Lisberger, & Bialek,
2005; Spering & Montagnini,
2011), facilitating transfer from motion perception to motion tracking. Even though there is evidence for interdependency between the neural control of eye and hand movements, particularly within posterior parietal cortex (Snyder, Batista, & Andersen,
1997; Buneo & Andersen,
2006; Cui & Andersen,
2007; Battaglia-Mayer, Ferrari-Toniolo, & Visco-Comandini,
2015), both types of movement are ultimately controlled by effector-specific networks. Moreover, there is little research on the neural mechanisms underlying pursuit-hand coordination, and the extent of overlap between the cortical architecture underlying each type of movement is unclear. Finally, our current task was more complex and required not only processing of sensory motion information, but also trajectory prediction, based on past experience. Lack of transfer could indicate that the process of integrating sensory with experience-based information might differ for pursuit and hand movements.