Multiple factors are known to play a role in emmetropization, including the structure of the environment (Morgan & Rose,
2005; Sherwin et al.,
2012; Wu, Tsai, Wu, Yang, & Kuo,
2013), the characteristics of the light (Rucker, Britton, Spatcher, & Hanowsky,
2015), and even the amount of exercise (Jacobsen, Jenson, & Goldschmidt,
2008; Thykjaer, Lundberg, & Grauslund,
2016). It is also known that the visual input to the retina plays a fundamental role, as evidenced by a vast body of work showing that emmetropic eyes actively change size to compensate for the insertion of lenses (Wildsoet,
1997; Tran, Chiu, Tian, & Wildsoet,
2008). Given this strong influence, a major focus of research has been on the possible roles of spatial cues, such as the optical quality of the retinal image and the magnitude/sign of blur (
Figure 1A; Wallman & Winawer,
2004). Yet correction of optical blur per se is not always sufficient to stop eye growth (Aller & Wildsoet,
2013), and in humans the eye often continues to elongate even when the image has been optically corrected.