First, as in Experiment 1, adaptation caused a reliable shift in the psychometric function toward the adapting stimulus (
Figure 6A, left; PSE shift for happy-adapted condition, pooled data, −0.073 ± 0.002,
p < 10
−8; median for individual subjects, −0.071; for all subjects
p ≤ 0.05; PSE shift for sad-adapted condition, pooled data 0.054 ± 0.002,
p < 10
−8; median for individual subjects, 0.051; for all subjects
p ≤ 0.05). Second, as in Experiment 1, adaptation caused a reduction in response time (
Figure 6A, right), which the diffusion model attributed to a reduction of decision bound with adaptation (a reduction of 11.4% for the happy-adapted condition,
p = 2.2 × 10
−7, and 6.8% for the sad-adapted condition,
p = 8.5 × 10
−4, compared to the unadapted condition; single subject results: happy-adapted condition, median 12.2%, for five out of six subjects
p ≤ 0.05, bootstrap
p < 10
−4 for combined changes across subjects; sad-adapted condition, median 10.1%, for four subjects
p ≤ 0.05, bootstrap
p < 10
−4 combined changes across subjects). This reduction in decision bound was slightly less pronounced than in Experiment 1, and unlike Experiment 1, there was little flattening of the psychometric function (
Display Formula\({\beta _4}\) = −0.41 ± 1.0,
p = 0.68,
Equation 2). Furthermore, there was an asymmetry in the decision bounds following adaptation (
Figure 7). We observed a lower bound for sad judgments in the happy-adapted condition (pooled data, 22.2 ± 0.5 vs. 26.4 ± 0.5,
p < 10
−8; median bound reduction for individual subjects, 12.8%; the reduction was significant for five out of six subjects with
p ≤ 0.05; combined changes across subjects, bootstrap
p < 10
−4), and a lower bound for happy judgments in the sad-adapted condition (23.9 ± 0.6 vs. 26.6 ± 0.6,
p = 1.7 × 10
−6; median bound reduction for individual subjects, 10.3%; the reduction was significant for four subjects with
p ≤ 0.05; combined changes across subjects, bootstrap
p < 10
−4).