Reward, attention, and arousal have highly overlapping brain circuitries (Maunsell,
2004; Schultz,
2006). In these studies, high reward might have improved perceptual performance and perceptual learning either through direct reward circuits or through associated changes in arousal and/or top-down attention (Della Libera & Chelazzi,
2006; Peck et al.,
2009). Indeed, some other researchers have also suggested that perceptual learning may improve performance through improved attention and/or arousal (Ahissar & Hochstein,
1997; Gilbert, Sigman, & Crist,
2001; Xiao et al.,
2008). The high-reward condition consisted of trial-by-trial rewards that may provide a trial-by-trial differential signal between predicted and received reward on the scale of seconds (Nomoto, Schultz, Watanabe, & Sakagami,
2010; Schultz,
1998), and between-block and between-session rewards may generate elevated arousal or top-down attention on the scale of minutes or hours (Roesch & Olson,
2007). If perceptual learning in the high-reward condition was driven only by improved top-down attention or arousal, one might expect the same amount of performance improvement in the trained and untrained eyes because top-down attention and arousal operate binocularly (Karni & Sagi,
1991; Schwartz, Maquet, & Frith,
2002). This was not the case. In fact, we found that the magnitude of the improvements in the AULCSF in the trained eye was significantly greater than that in the untrained eye in the high-reward condition in
Experiment 1. The result suggests that perceptual learning in the high-reward condition was not driven solely by improved top-down attention or arousal. In the block-reward condition, no trial-by-trial reward was available, but observers improved their performance more than in the no- and low-reward conditions. The partial but lower efficacy of block reward parallels the results with block compared to trial-by-trial feedback (Herzog & Fahle,
1997; Liu et al.,
2014; Shibata, Yamagishi, Ishii, & Kawato,
2009). We speculate that block reward may influence learning through self-assessed aspects of performance (Liu et al.,
2014) or through changes in arousal or effort. Trial-by-trial subliminal reward enhanced perceptual learning only slightly less than the high-reward condition, consistent with other findings that unconscious reward could improve perceptual learning (Xue et al.,
2015). Finally, explicit trial-by-trial high reward generated the greatest amount of perceptual learning, suggesting that conscious awareness of reward may amplify its effects slightly and further enhance perceptual learning (Zedelius, Veling, & Aarts,
2012). Taken together, these results suggest the structure of reward itself is the largest determinant of perceptual learning and transfer.