Interestingly, there are stages in healthy observers during which the perceptual system is also particularly fragile or immature. For example, from early infancy to late adolescence, the brain undergoes a wide array of anatomical and functional changes as it develops (e.g., Blakemore,
2012; Blakemore & Choudhury,
2006; Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston,
2005; Durston et al.,
2001). Similarly, during normal aging, the cognitive functions decline, which is induced by age-related loss of synaptic contacts, neural apoptosis (e.g., Raz,
2000; Rossini, Rossi, Babiloni, & Polich,
2007), reduction in cerebral blood flow (e.g., Chen, Rosas, & Salat,
2011), or volume reduction in different brain regions (e.g., amygdala, hippocampus, frontal cortex; Calder et al.,
2003; C. R. Jack et al.,
1997; Navarro & Gonzalo,
1991; Ruffman, Henry, Livingstone, & Phillips,
2008). Considering the increased vulnerability of the brain under neural architectural changes (Andersen,
2003; Hof & Morrison,
2004), it is possible that healthy young children and normal aging adults also benefit from the presentation of dynamic faces. However, only a few developmental studies have compared facial expression recognition in children using both static and dynamic stimuli (Nelson, Hudspeth, & Russell,
2013; Nelson & Russell,
2011). These studies yielded equivocal results, none of them revealing a significant advantage for dynamic over static stimuli; two studies even pointed to differences favoring static stimuli (Nelson & Russell,
2011; Widen & Russell,
2015). Nevertheless, most of these studies tested facial expression recognition with the use of a single actor and provided additional information about face, body movements, and vocal intonations, which may have facilitated expression recognition. In the aging literature, a small number of studies examined facial expression recognition with static and dynamic faces (Grainger, Henry, Phillips, Vanman, & Allen,
2015; Krendl & Ambady,
2010; Sze, Goodkind, Gyurak, & Levenson,
2012). Although most of these studies pointed to a dynamic advantage for the recognition of facial expressions, they (a) did not use a database of static and dynamic stimuli controlled for the amount of
low–level visual information carried over time (Grainger et al.,
2015; Sze et al.,
2012), (b) were limited to a subset of emotional expressions (Krendl & Ambady,
2010), (c) included participants in only one condition (Krendl & Ambady,
2010), or (d) relied on dynamic movies that were not displaying natural expressions (Grainger et al.,
2015). These methodological issues considerably limit firm conclusions on the potential benefits of dynamic cues for the recognition of facial expressions in elderly people.