Neuroimaging studies support the notion of overlapping neural networks for attention and eye movements (Beauchamp, Petit, Ellmore, Ingeholm, & Haxby,
2001; Corbetta et al.,
1998; Nobre, Gitelman, Dias, & Mesulam,
2000) and the idea of a functional link between attention and movements is well established in the literature (McFadden, Khan, & Wallman,
2002). Using dual-task paradigms in which participants perform prosaccades and discriminate symbols at the same time, several studies have confirmed the linkage between attention and saccades. These studies showed that visual perception is facilitated specifically at the future location of the upcoming saccade (Castet, Jeanjean, Montagnini, Laugier, & Masson,
2006; Deubel & Schneider,
1996; Hoffman & Subramaniam,
1995; Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser,
1995), with attention preceding saccade onset by approximately 50 ms (Deubel,
2008; Rolfs, Jonikaitis, Deubel, & Cavanagh,
2011; Rolfs & Carrasco,
2012). There are two main explanatory theories for this linkage. The premotor theory of attention posits a strong coupling between goal-directed actions and shifts of attention (Kustov & Robinson,
1996; Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umiltá,
1987; Rizzolatti, Riggio, & Sheliga,
1994; Sheliga, Riggio, & Rizzolatti,
1994) because attention allocation and saccade preparation rely on the same mechanisms of the eye movement system. According to the premotor theory, shifting attention to an object is equivalent to planning a saccade that is not executed. In other words, saccade preparation is responsible for the shift of attention to the location of the planned response. Importantly, this leads to the prediction that if the saccade planned is too long or too short, attention allocation should also show the same offset relative to the target. On the other hand, the visual system is anatomically divided in two streams: The ventral stream is associated with object recognition, whereas the dorsal stream is involved in sensorimotor transformations for visually guided actions toward this object (Goodale & Milner,
1992). The visual attention model assumes the existence of a common object selection mechanism for ventral and dorsal processing (Schneider,
1995) but the dorsal system processes its extrinsic spatial features and especially its location in eye-centered reference frame for saccade planning while the ventral system processes its intrinsic features for perceptual categorization processes. According to this single selection mechanism, attentional resources for perception and for action would be allocated to the same spatial location, but an error in saccade planning should not necessarily be reflected in attentional shifts because attention is directed to the intended rather than to the actual saccadic landing position (Deubel & Schneider,
1996; Schneider,
1995).