Previous studies have shown that specific eye movements were not necessarily required for causal impression of collision events (Badler, Lefèvre, & Missal,
2012; Jansson,
1964). Apart from causal impression, was there any possibility in
Experiments 2 to
4 that the contact order of the COs was influenced by any eye movement that followed the COs or EO? In the present experiments, the participants were required to focus on the fixation point, and it was unlikely that eye movement was systemically involved in contact order judgment. Also, there are some reasons suggesting that eye movement does not account for our results, even if we accept that eye movement occurred. One could argue that the participant strategically followed one of the COs and therefore may have judged the CO to contact the EO first because of attentional benefits for the perceptual process of the CO, as found in previous studies (McDonald, Teder-Sälejärvi, & Hillyard,
2000; Stelmach & Herdman,
1991). However, although both COs start to move separately, they converge at the EO around the collision area, and they are closely formed on the fovea. Thus, any overt or covert attentional benefit based on location would occur only rarely. Furthermore, this account cannot explain why the attentional benefit occurs only for the CO that has good continuation with the EO. What if the participants follow the EO and are asked to judge the contact order of the COs? In previous studies in which the participants were asked to actively follow the EO, the latency of saccadic eye movement for following the EO was faster in the physically possible causal condition than in the physically impossible causal condition (Badler, Lefèvre, & Missal,
2010; Wende, Theunissen, & Missal,
2016). Accordingly, if the participants in our study judged the contact order of the COs based on eye movement latency, the CO that showed good continuation with the EO would be judged to contact the EO first, as they would have a stronger causal relation. However, unlike those previous studies, there were two COs in the present experiments, such that it would be difficult to expect any CO to show good continuation. Also, in those previous studies, the latencies of saccadic eye movement were greater than 150 ms after collision. Good continuation effects in the present experiments were observed only with a much shorter time delay to the start of the EO motion, and the accuracy was almost perfect when the delay was greater than 120 ms. Badler et al. (
2012) also found that the latencies of saccadic eye movement following the EO did not differ between the 0-ms delay condition and 300-ms delay condition when the EO showed a physically possible motion pathway, though the causal impression differed as casual impression versus noncausal impressions, such as with the motion of two objects. Taken together, it seems unlikely that the good continuation effect observed in the present study can be accounted for by eye movement.