The perception of translucence has been relatively little studied, in contrast to work on other visual properties such as color. Metelli (
1970), while not the first to study the phenomenon, made one of the first significant attempts to explain perceived transparency (the absorption component of the more general property of translucence). He outlined potential algebraic conditions under which we perceive transparency, in terms of relative figural relationships and ordinal relationships between colors in a scene. However, this was only relevant to specific limited contexts and materials. Subsequent work until the early 2000s went on to define other possible comparative relationships between materials in terms of conditions that might have to be satisfied for the visual system to define a surface as transparent. This work produced descriptive laws but did not attempt to explain how we perceive the translucence itself (Adelson & Anandan,
1990; Beck, Prazdny, & Ivry,
1984; Singh & Hoffman,
1998). Theories of translucence developed further as technology improved, with research showing that it had many other important aspects (Jensen, Marschner, Levoy, & Hanrahan,
2001; Koenderink & van Doorn,
2001; Singh & Anderson,
2002a,
2002b). It also became clear that most published work did not attempt to address the fundamental questions of how translucence was perceived, although it was evident that observers were very good at discriminating and identifying characteristics of translucence, like opacity, at short presentation times (Sharan, Rosenholtz, & Adelson,
2009). Fleming and colleagues concluded that many of the simple cues that had been proposed for the perception of translucence were unable to predict how the translucence of a material was perceived (Fleming & Bülthoff,
2005; Fleming, Torralba, & Adelson,
2004). They argued that the physics of translucence—much like the physics of surface reflectance, in color vision—were too complex for the visual system to be able to estimate using inverse optics (i.e., working backward from the stimulus received to the material most likely to have produced it based on mental models of light transport). In addition, those authors argued that simple image statistics were inadequate when used alone. Nevertheless, they proposed that perceived translucence was achieved by means of parsing scenes into key regions and gathering image statistics from those regions, though the precise statistics remained unknown.