Simultaneous two-dimensional Bayesian approaches can estimate a participant's CSF in less than five minutes (Dorr et al.,
2017) and can even be run on a portable device (Dorr, Lesmes, Lu, & Bex,
2013), but they do not address one of the fundamental restrictions of traditional psychophysical tasks: their reliance on extended periods of attention and volitional perceptual report. The tasks can be tedious because they require the repeated presentation of visually uninteresting stimuli such as filtered noise patterns and sinusoidal gratings. Adaptive procedures also naturally become more difficult as the threshold of an observer's ability is approached; by design, these tasks spend as much time as possible presenting stimuli at peri-threshold combinations of contrast and spatial frequency. These drawbacks can be tolerated by healthy adults, but are not well tolerated by observers who are less willing or less able to engage with the test, such as children or individuals with brain injury (Witton, Talcott, & Henning,
2017). Witton et al. suggest that the only available workaround for less motivated subjects has been to add more trials (hence, more time) to the task. This strategy, however, does not help individuals (of any age) who have difficulty sustaining attention, following task instructions, or communicating their responses to an experimenter. Simplified report-based tools such as Pelli-Robson charts (Pelli, Robson, & Wilkins,
1988) can provide fast approximations to full psychophysical measurement of contrast sensitivity for certain ranges of spatial frequencies (Leat & Woo,
1997), but are similarly reliant on intentional feedback from the participant. There are alternatives to report-based tasks for impaired, noncommunicative populations, but they also have significant shortcomings that have prevented them from being routinely used in clinical settings. Preferential looking paradigms such as Teller cards circumvent the need for verbal communication, but they are still highly dependent on the participant's attention span (Teller, McDonald, Preston, Sebris, & Dobson,
1986). Electrophysiological methods such as visual evoked potentials are more sensitive than preferential looking paradigms (Katsumi, Denno, Arai, de Faria, & Hirose,
1997; Riddell et al.,
1997) and have the significant benefit of providing objective measures of visual function (Leat, Yadav, & Irving,
2009), but require specialized training to administer and have reduced sensitivity relative to tasks with intentional behavioral report (de Faria, Katsumi, Arai, & Hirose,
1998). The longer time required for setup and measurement may also make it more difficult for participants to consistently attend to the display throughout the task, particularly if they have cognitive impairments. More promising techniques for assessing these populations have emerged that infer stimulus visibility indirectly through tracking behavior. Bonnen, Burge, Yates, Pillow, and Cormack (
2015) had participants move a cursor to track the position of a luminance patch obscured by noise and found that tracking precision could be used to estimate visual uncertainty as accurately as (and more efficiently than) perceptual report. The authors describe their approach as “continuous psychophysics,” in which every change in the stimulus constitutes an informative “mini-trial.” Continuous measures of visual function have similarly been obtained using eye trackers (see Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner,
2011), which have been rapidly improving in accuracy and accessibility (Gibaldi, Vanegas, Bex, & Maiello,
2017). Most of these studies used eye trackers to assess low-level oculomotor function (such as saccade latency; Engbert & Kliegl,
2003) or to infer attention in subjective preferential looking tasks aimed at higher-order abilities such as object perception (Einhäuser, Spain, & Perona,
2008) and face perception (Cerf, Frady, & Koch,
2009).