Accurately estimating the speed of moving objects and self-motion with respect to the world is an important task for the nervous system which supports safe locomotion and interaction with the environment. This crucial ability depends on transformations between different reference frames, including retinal, head, and world coordinate systems. Extraretinal signals about eye movements must be taken into account to transform retinal signals into a head-centric reference frame and estimate object speed relative to the head (von Holst & Mittelstaedt,
1950). Similarly, when the head moves, vestibular and neck-muscle information must be used to transform signals from head-centered to body- and world-centered reference frames.
The estimates resulting from these transformations can be biased, yielding phenomena such as the Filehne illusion in which stationary objects appear to move during smooth pursuit eye movements (Filehne,
1922). An analogous vestibular phenomenon is the oculogyral illusion, in which a head-fixed visual target seems to move during physical self-rotation relative to the observer in the direction of angular acceleration (Graybiel & Hupp,
1946).
The Filehne illusion is thought to arise from differences in the perceptual estimates of retinal and oculomotor speed (Freeman, Champion, & Warren,
2010; Furman & Gur,
2012; Haarmeier & Thier,
1996; Souman, Hooge, & Wertheim,
2005,
2006; Wertheim,
1981,
1987). This mismatch leads also to a related bias in perceived speed called the Aubert-Fleischl phenomenon (AF), in which pursued targets are perceived to move more slowly than nonpursued ones (Aubert,
1886). The underestimation of object speed during oculomotor pursuit was originally attributed to an erroneous estimate of eye velocity via an extraretinal signal, whereas retinal motion estimates were hypothesized to be veridical (Mack & Herman,
1973; Raymond, Shapiro, & Rose,
1984). Subsequent work cast doubt on this hypothesis by showing that the AF and Filehne illusion can be reversed, i.e., retinal velocity may become underestimated compared to oculomotor velocity, depending on the spatial frequency of the stimulus (Freeman & Banks,
1998; Wertheim,
1987). In other words, the strength and direction of the AF depends on the relationship between retinal and oculomotor speed estimates, with the former being a function of the stimulus. If both signals are linearly related to speed, then their ratio captures the behavior of phenomena such as the AF and Filehne illusion (Freeman,
2001; Furman & Gur,
2012; Souman et al.,
2006).
In addition to eye movement, head and body movement also lead to motion at the retina, so the question arises how other reference frame transformations, e.g., into body or world coordinates, influence the perception of object speed. When the head moves, head-centric estimates can be transformed into a world-centric coordinate system using additional cues, i.e., signals from the vestibular system. These carry information about linear and angular accelerations and thereby allow for estimation of head motion.
In summary, estimation of object motion when the observers move their eyes, head, and body can be recovered by integrating the speed of the object on the retina, the speed of the eyes with respect to the head, and the movement of the head in space. Previous studies have focused on the perception of object speed in experimental conditions where the head was held still while the eyes were either fixating a stationary target or pursuing a moving target (Dichgans, Wist, Diener, & Brandt,
1975; Freeman,
2001; Freeman & Banks,
1998; Freeman et al.,
2010; Powell, Meredith, McMillin, & Freeman,
2016; Raymond et al.,
1984; Souman et al.,
2006; Wertheim,
1987). The present study, in contrast, investigates the impact of vestibular signals on the perception of perceived object motion in the world during passive whole-body rotations.
There are some previous studies that have investigated the impact of vestibular signals on perception of object motion (e.g., Dyde & Harris,
2008; Jaekl, Jenkin, & Harris,
2005). However, these studies were not conducted under conditions that allowed for direct comparison between vestibular and oculomotor compensation which was our intention here. To preview our findings, we observe a phenomenon analogous to the classical (oculomotor) AF, in which objects that are pursued with a whole-body rotation, appear to move more slowly than nonpursued objects. We call this the vestibular AF. Our results show that the oculomotor and vestibular AF effects are similar in magnitude and can be described by a simple linear model of the signals involved.