Visual working memory (VWM) supports the integration of past and present sensory information via short-term maintenance when such information is no longer directly accessible. Performance on VWM tasks is highly correlated with measures of general intelligence and other related outcome measures and is therefore thought to reflect a core cognitive capacity (Baddeley,
1986; Conway, Cowan, Bunting, Therriault, & Minkoff,
2002; Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway,
1999; Fukuda, Vogel, Mayr, & Awh,
2010). In most VWM studies, simple visual stimuli are presented on a 2-D computer screen and participants remember specific features, such as color or orientation, that are presented at different spatial locations (Engle et al.,
1999; Luck & Vogel,
1997; Simons & Levin,
1997; Zhang & Luck,
2008). Based on such work, VWM is known to be limited in capacity (Bays, Catalao, & Husain,
2009; Bays & Husain,
2008; Ma, Husain, & Bays,
2014; Schurgin, Wixted, & Brady,
2018), such that increasing the number of items to be remembered or the delay duration leads to reductions in memory precision (Ma et al.,
2014; Panichello, DePasquale, Pillow, & Buschman,
2018; Rademaker, Park, & Sack,
2018; Shin, Zou, & Ma,
2017; van den Berg, Shin, Chou, George, & Ma,
2012; Zhang & Luck,
2008), reductions in confidence (Rademaker, Tredway, & Tong,
2012), the misbinding or “swapping” of different visual features (Bays,
2016; Bays, Gorgoraptis, Wee, Marshall, & Husain,
2011; Bays, Wu, & Husain,
2011), and the tendency to chunk information into group-level ensemble representations (Brady & Alvarez,
2011).