Human visual areas hMT+ and hV4 are regions within the dorsal and ventral processing streams, respectively, that have each been attributed with distinct visual specializations: global motion selectivity for hMT+ and shape processing for hV4 (Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983b; Newsome & Pare,
1988; Gallant, Braun, & van Essen,
1993; Born & Bradley,
2005; Rust, Mante, Simoncelli, & Movshon,
2006; Roe et al.,
2012; Yue, Pourladian, Tootell, & Ungerleider,
2014). Although a conventional view of visual processing assumes the existence of neural substructures in extrastriate cortex with well-defined and independent functional properties, there is growing evidence against a strict segregation of function, indicative of more integrative processing with functional overlap between regions. Functional connectivity, for instance, is known to exist between ventral visual areas and the lateral temporal motion sensitive regions, for example, between MT and V2/V4 (Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983a; Ungerleider & Desimone,
1986; Walsh, Ellison, Battelli, & Cowey,
1998). Moreover, although motion inputs are undoubtedly processed in motion-sensitive cortical regions, such as MT, MST, and V3A (Zeki,
1974; Newsome & Pare,
1988; McKeefry, Burton, Vakrou, Barrett, & Morland,
2008), a range of findings suggest that ventral cortical regions may also be implicated in the perception of motion and appear to exhibit some degree of motion sensitivity (Ungerleider & Desimone,
1986; Newsome,
1997; P. Thompson, Brooks, & Hammett,
2006; Hayward, Truong, Partanen, & Giaschi,
2011). Results from nonhuman primates indicate that slower motion might be supported by the ventral pathway given the sustained nature of the parvocellular system, whereas faster motion may be supported by the more transient magnocellular pathway (Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983a; Ungerleider & Desimone,
1986; Ferrera, Nealey, & Maunsell,
1994; Lu, Chen, Tanigawa, & Roe,
2010). In addition, electrophysiology in macaque ventral cortex has revealed motion-selective pathways that project to ventral cortical regions with a significant proportion of neurons in V4 exhibiting direction selectivity (Desimone & Schein,
1987; Ferrera et al.,
1994; Tolias, Sultan, et al.,
2005; Gur & Snodderly,
2007; Schmid et al.,
2013). Optical imaging has also shown macaque V4 to contain a columnar organization of motion-directional maps sensitive to changes in motion direction (Lu et al.,
2010; An et al.,
2012; Li et al.,
2013). Conversely, there is less evidence to suggest that hMT+ might play a role in the perception of static stimuli. In fact, this area has provided some of the strongest evidence in favor of a functional specialization of brain areas. Nonetheless, the presence of orientation-selective neurons in MT that respond to static stimuli suggests that this area may not be invariant to static form (Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983b; T. D. Albright,
1984; Newsome & Pare,
1988; Khawaja, Liu, & Pack,
2013). Human fMRI also implies responses in hMT+ to static stimuli (O'Craven, Rosen, Kwong, Treisman, & Savoy,
1997).