Open Access
Article  |   May 2019
Temporary monocular occlusion facilitates binocular fusion during rivalry
Author Affiliations
  • Yasha Sheynin
    McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
    jacob.sheynin@mail.mcgill.ca
  • Sébastien Proulx
    McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
  • Robert F. Hess
    McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
Journal of Vision May 2019, Vol.19, 23. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/19.5.23
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Yasha Sheynin, Sébastien Proulx, Robert F. Hess; Temporary monocular occlusion facilitates binocular fusion during rivalry. Journal of Vision 2019;19(5):23. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.5.23.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

A few hours of monocular patching temporarily enhances the deprived eye's contribution to binocular vision, constituting a form of adult brain plasticity. Although the mechanism for this plasticity is currently unknown, several imaging studies present evidence that monocular deprivation achieves its effects by changing excitatory–inhibitory balance in the visual cortex. Much of the past work on adult monocular patching utilized binocular rivalry to quantify the patching-induced shift in perceptual eye dominance, extracting periods of exclusive visibility (in which one eye's signal is suppressed from perception) to assess each eye's contribution to binocular vision while overlooking the occurrence of mixed visibility (in which information from both eyes is combined). In this paper, we discuss two experiments to investigate the effects of short-term monocular occlusion on the relative predominance of mixed and exclusive percepts during binocular rivalry. In addition to the known perceptual eye-dominance shift, we hypothesized patching would also increase the perception of mixtures during rivalry due to deprivation-induced changes in excitatory–inhibitory balance. Our data point to two previously unknown effects of monocular deprivation: (a) a significant increase in the overall fraction and median duration of mixed visibility during rivalry that is detectable up to at least an hour after removing the patch and (b) the overall fraction of superimposition; rather than piecemeal, mixed percepts are specifically enhanced after monocular deprivation. In addition to strengthening the contribution of the deprived eye, our results show that temporary monocular patching enhances the visibility of fused binocular percepts, likely the result of attenuated interocular inhibition.

Introduction
Short-term monocular deprivation (MD) is known to have several effects on adult human vision (see Baldwin & Hess, 2018, for an overview). MD, or patching, can shift perceptual eye dominance at the neural level (Hubel & Wiesel, 1970; Tso, Miller, & Begum, 2017). In childhood, long-term (>1 week) MD causes a shift in favor of the nondeprived eye, and temporarily patching an eye for a few hours in adulthood results in a shift in favor the deprived eye that is observable up to at least an hour after deprivation (Lunghi, Burr, & Morrone, 2011). The ability of the adult visual system to temporarily shift perceptual eye dominance points to a latent functional plasticity whose mechanism is currently unknown although there is empirical evidence implicating changes in excitatory–inhibitory (E-I) balance in V1 (Chadnova, Reynaud, Clavagnier, & Hess, 2017; Lunghi, Emir, Morrone, & Bridge, 2015). 
Studies investigating the effects of MD on binocular vision generally measure perceptual eye dominance behaviorally with either binocular rivalry (Binda et al., 2017; Kim, Kim, & Blake, 2017; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015; Lunghi, Morrone, Secci, & Caputo, 2016) or binocular phase combination (Baldwin & Hess, 2018; Chadnova et al., 2017; Zhou, Baker, Simard, Saint-Amour, & Hess, 2015) tasks. For the purpose of the current paper, we focus on binocular rivalry. Binocular rivalry occurs when the eyes are presented separate incongruent images, and it is defined by perceptual alternations that shift perception from one eye's image to the other over the course of presentation (see Blake & Logothetis, 2002, for a review). Studies that use binocular rivalry to measure perceptual eye dominance infer the contributions of the two eyes from the degree to which one eye suppresses the other when competing, or rivaling, for perception. 
Although binocular rivalry is often characterized as a series of perceptual alternations between two competing images, the actual visual experience is more extensive and can be separated into three categories: (a) exclusive visibility, when one eye's signal is entirely suppressed by the other eye's image; (b) piecemeal mixed visibility in which information from both eyes is simultaneously visible in smaller spatially segregated areas, sometimes described as local rivalry (Skerswetat, Formankiewicz, & Waugh, 2017); and (c) superimposition mixed visibility in which information from both eyes is visible and combined to constitute a fused binocular percept (Brascamp, van Ee, Noest, Jacobs, & van den Berg, 2006; Liu, Tyler, & Schor, 1992). Importantly, mixed visibility highlights instances when complete interocular suppression fails, allowing binocular combination to occur. In fact, mixed visibility has been shown to be negatively associated with resting-state GABA levels in V1 (Freyberg, Robertson, & Baron-Cohen, 2015) and has likewise been shown to decrease after administration of GABA agonist drugs (Mentch, Spiegel, Ricciardi, Kanwisher, & Robertson, 2018). Disinhibition of interocular interactions are plausibly responsible for superimposition percepts, and piecemeal percepts are proposed to emerge from a weakening of the spatial coherence of these inhibitory interactions (Alais & Melcher, 2007; Kovacs, Papathomas, Yang, & Feher, 1996; Lee & Blake, 2004). 
Our study on the effects of patching was inspired, in part, from the finding that the predominance of mixed visibility can be altered in real time by recent visual experience (Klink, Brascamp, Blake, & Van Wezel, 2010). This finding demonstrated that the proportion of mixed percepts increases over the course of continuous exposure to rivalrous stimuli (found with both gratings and natural images) and, importantly, that presentation of nonrivalrous binocular stimuli is necessary to restore this proportion back to baseline levels. This study highlighted a central role for experience-driven plasticity in adult binocular vision, causally linking recent visual experience to changes in binocular rivalry dynamics. It may be useful to consider the effects of several hours of MD as a form of experience-driven plasticity. 
Furthermore, studies using binocular rivalry to investigate the effects of MD (Binda et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2017; Lunghi, Berchicci, Morrone, & Di Russo, 2015; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015) have not explored the role of mixed percepts in the effect of patching, focusing instead on using the exclusive percepts to quantify shifts in perceptual eye dominance. Although a previous rivalry study on patching (Lunghi et al., 2011) reported that their stimulus did not produce much mixed visibility (less than 20%), an earlier article (O'Shea, Sims, & Govan, 1997) highlighted up to 60% mixed visibility using similar stimulus parameters (1.5° diameter sinusoidal gratings, 3 c/°, presented dichoptically). This discrepancy may be due to differences in task instructions and response options; however, given that MD alters binocular rivalry dynamics and that the predominance of mixed visibility is known to be directly influenced by recent visual experience, we felt it would be pertinent to conduct a systematic investigation of patching-induced changes in rivalry dynamics using task instructions that require attending to mixed percepts. 
To do so, we designed two experiments that permitted us to simultaneously quantify patching-induced changes in perceptual eye dominance and mixed visibility. Specifically, Experiment 1 utilized a novel rivalry task to quantify patching-induced changes in five different rivalry percept states: the exclusive percepts of the left and right eye's images, the mixed percept biased in favor of the left and right eye's images, and a balanced mixture of the left and right eye's images. Our rationale for using this task design was to encourage participants not to classify mixed percepts biased in favor of one eye as an exclusive percept. This approach allowed us to more reliably estimate the relative predominance of mixed and exclusive visibility during rivalry while also allowing us to measure changes in perceptual eye dominance. 
Experiment 2 was a follow up to Experiment 1 to determine whether piecemeal or superimposition percepts were specifically targeted by the effects of deprivation. To investigate this, we used a task adapted from Skerswetat et al. (2017) that allowed us to simultaneously measure patching-induced changes in perceptual eye dominance as well as the relative predominance of superimposition and piecemeal percepts. 
Due to the findings that recent visual experience can alter binocular rivalry dynamics (Freyberg et al., 2015; Klink et al., 2010) and that monocular patching alters E-I balance in the visual cortex (Binda et al., 2017; Chadnova et al., 2017; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015), we predicted that patching would significantly increase the proportion and median duration of mixed percepts while simultaneously shifting perceptual eye dominance in favor of the deprived eye. Likewise, under the assumption that patching weakens interocular inhibition, we predicted that patching would selectively increase the proportion and median duration of superimposition rather than piecemeal percepts. 
Experiment 1
We designed Experiment 1 to investigate the effects of short-term monocular patching on mixed visibility during rivalry. This experiment used a five-alternative, forced-choice (5AFC) binocular rivalry task to evaluate patching-induced changes in rivalry dynamics along a discretized spectrum of percept states that ranged from the exclusive percepts from the left eye's image to that from the right eye's image, including three intermediate mixed percept states. 
Methods and materials
Observers
A total of 16 individuals enrolled in Experiment 1 (eight women, 22 ± 2.3, one author). Two participants were excluded from the study due to data-collection errors during baseline measurements, and one participant was excluded because the participant's median rivalry phase durations at baseline were greater than 4 SD of the group mean. In sum, 13 individuals participated in the study. A subset of our participants (N = 5, three women, 24 ± 1.3) completed additional postdeprivation measurements that were taken over the course of an hour after removing the eye patch to evaluate the decay of the patching-induced changes in rivalry dynamics. 
All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and were free from ocular diseases. Normal stereovision was confirmed through the Randot task. This research was approved by the ethics review board of the McGill University Health Center and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Subjects gave written informed consent prior to the experiment. All participants except for the author YS were naive to the purpose of the experiment. 
Apparatus
Each session took place in a quiet room with dim light. Visual stimuli for the binocular rivalry experiments were generated and controlled by an Apple MacBook Pro 2008 computer (MacOSX; Cupertino, CA) running MATLAB R2012B (MathWorks, Natick, MA) with the Psychtoolbox psychophysics toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner, Brainard & Pelli, 2007; Pelli, 1997). Stimuli were displayed on a wide 2300 3-D ready LED monitor ViewSonic V3D231, gamma corrected with a mean luminance of 100 cd/m2. Subjects viewed the stimuli at a viewing distance of 70 cm with passive polarized 3-D glasses so that the left image was only seen by the left eye and the right image by the right eye. The polarized filters had the effect of reducing the luminance to about 40%, measured with a photometer. 
The stereo image input was in top-down VGA format and was displayed in interleaved line stereo mode at a resolution of 1,920 × 1,080 p and a refresh rate of 60 Hz: the left-eye image was displayed in even scanlines, and the right-eye image was displayed in odd scanlines. Crosstalk levels for polarizing filter and passive goggle systems, such as the one we used are known to be low (luminance crosstalk: 1.14%, CI: [1.13, 1.15], contrast crosstalk: −0.04%, CI: [−0.28, 0.18]; Baker, Kaestner, & Gouws, 2016). 
Stimulus
The dichoptic stimulus was composed of two orthogonal (±45°) sinusoidal gratings. These gratings were 3 c/°, subtending a diameter of 1.5° with a raised cosine annulus blurring the edges, Michelson contrast = 75%, presented inside a black-and-white noise pattern frame (side = 10°; Figure 1A). 
Figure 1
 
Methods. (A) Experimental protocol. Baseline rivalry data was obtained from four 180-s rivalry blocks, each consisting of two 90-s rivalry runs. The first block of the baseline measurements was discarded. The baseline measurements were calculated by extracting the median of the three remaining blocks. Following baseline testing, we patched the participants' nondominant eye with a diffuser eye patch for 2 hr. After this, we continued with three postpatching rivalry blocks over the course of 9 min after removing the patch and extracted our main postpatching measurement by taking the median of these blocks. (B) Baseline data. Median phase durations (left) and overall fractions (right) (M ± SEM) for the five percept states obtained using the binocular rivalry task in Experiment 1. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (C) Experiment 1: 5AFC binocular rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (ML) a mostly left-oriented grating with some right-oriented lines, (M) a balanced left- and right-oriented grating (indicated by the absence of a key press), (MR) a mostly right-oriented grating with some left-oriented lines, or (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating. (D) Experiment 2: 4AFC superimposition versus piecemeal rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating, pressing both (L + R) simultaneously to indicate they were seeing a piecemeal percept, or (M) a superimposition percept.
Figure 1
 
Methods. (A) Experimental protocol. Baseline rivalry data was obtained from four 180-s rivalry blocks, each consisting of two 90-s rivalry runs. The first block of the baseline measurements was discarded. The baseline measurements were calculated by extracting the median of the three remaining blocks. Following baseline testing, we patched the participants' nondominant eye with a diffuser eye patch for 2 hr. After this, we continued with three postpatching rivalry blocks over the course of 9 min after removing the patch and extracted our main postpatching measurement by taking the median of these blocks. (B) Baseline data. Median phase durations (left) and overall fractions (right) (M ± SEM) for the five percept states obtained using the binocular rivalry task in Experiment 1. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (C) Experiment 1: 5AFC binocular rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (ML) a mostly left-oriented grating with some right-oriented lines, (M) a balanced left- and right-oriented grating (indicated by the absence of a key press), (MR) a mostly right-oriented grating with some left-oriented lines, or (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating. (D) Experiment 2: 4AFC superimposition versus piecemeal rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating, pressing both (L + R) simultaneously to indicate they were seeing a piecemeal percept, or (M) a superimposition percept.
Monocular deprivation
Using the Miles (1930) test for sensory eye dominance, we identified the dominant eye for each participant. We confirmed perceptual eye dominance with baseline binocular rivalry data for each subject (Dieter, Sy, & Blake, 2016) and proceeded to patch the nondominant eye for each experimental session in which they participated. We chose to patch the nondominant eye with the rationale that it has more capacity to increase its dominance; however, this claim has not yet been systematically evaluated. We used a diffuser eye patch that preserved most luminance information (40% luminance reduction) but eliminated all pattern information as confirmed by a Fourier decomposition of a natural image viewed through the patch. Although most studies use a patching duration of 2.5 hr, recent investigations have shown comparable effects after 2 hr of patching (Lunghi et al., 2016). To minimize the amount of time it would take to complete a single session, we monocularly deprived the nondominant eye for 2 hr. 
Binocular rivalry task and experimental protocol
We designed the 5AFC binocular rivalry task used in Experiment 1 to extract more reliable information about rivalry dynamics than the conventional two (left vs. right) or three (left vs. mixed vs. right) AFC approach (Figure 1C). Reports of lower-than-expected levels of mixed visibility at baseline in other 2AFC or 3AFC rivalry studies (Lunghi et al., 2011) using similar stimulus parameters could be attributed to the fact that participants begin to miscategorize their rivalry percepts, reporting a mixed percept biased in favor of one eye's image as that eye's exclusive percept. Our task design stresses attention to the phenomenological difference between mixed and exclusive percepts. An earlier article regarding the effect of stimulus parameters on the predominance of mixed visibility (O'Shea et al., 1997) was reported to be approximately 40% (SF: 3 c/°, field size: 1.5°). Our data set produced a similar figure with an average fraction of mixed visibility at baseline at 42% ± 5.76% (SEM). 
At the beginning of each session, participants were told that they would see a dynamic stimulus during the experiment and that their task was to track what they were seeing with particular attention to timeliness and accuracy. Participants were given an illustration (Figure 1C) of the types of stimuli they would be seeing so as to better categorize their responses during the task. 
Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing either (a) an exclusively left-tilted grating, (b) a mixed but predominantly left-tilted grating, (c) a mixed but predominantly right-tilted grating, or (d) an exclusively right-tilted grating, using four separate keys. In our instructions, we specified that exclusive percepts were those with 90% or more left- or right-tilted lines and the mixed percepts were between 50% and 90% left- or right-tilted lines. If the participants could not discriminate a mixed percept as predominantly left or right oriented, they were instructed not to respond, constituting our “balanced” mixed percept state. 
Each rivalry measurement began with a dichoptic nonius cross presented inside a 3° oval surrounded by a black-and-white noise (1 c/°) frame (side = 10°). The observer was asked to make key presses to adjust the position of the two frames to calibrate the optimal position for comfortable fusion. After confirmation, the participant was instructed to fixate at a fixation dot (0.2°) and place their hands on the appropriate keys to begin responding to the rivalry task. After a key press, the dichoptic stimulus appeared, and participants began responding to what they were observing on the monitor using the key press instructions provided at the beginning of each session. Subsequent blocks were initiated after a brief break in which subjects viewed a mean gray background screen. Subjects performed blocks of the binocular rivalry task before and after 2 hr of MD of the nondominant eye. During deprivation, subjects were instructed to keep both eyes open and do normal activities, such as watching a movie or doing computer work in a well-lit room. 
All participants were trained with up to five rivalry training blocks before beginning baseline measurements. We provided a break of 15 min between training and baseline blocks. Baseline measurements were drawn from four 180-s rivalry blocks, each consisting of two 90-s rivalry runs. The orientation of the gratings seen by the eyes was flipped between the two runs in each block to counterbalance possible orientation-eye biases and to interrupt any possible adaptation effects that would result in an increase in mixed visibility (Klink et al., 2010). We discarded the first rivalry block to account for possible errors made in the beginning of the task. All participants completed three postpatching measurements over the course of 9 min after patching. Five subjects completed additional postpatching rivalry blocks conducted at 30 and 60 min after removing the eye patch. 
Preprocessing and statistical analysis
The goal of preprocessing the raw rivalry time series data was to extract key features of the data usable for our analyses. Our main points of interest for analysis were patching-induced differences in (a) the median durations of the percept states, defined as the median of the distribution of durations spent perceiving each percept category; (b) the overall fraction of each percept state; and (c) perceptual eye dominance, defined as the ratio of the total durations spent viewing one eye's exclusive percept versus the other's. 
The preprocessing pipeline consisted of four stages: (a) remove the first and last percept states in the time series as well as all percept states shorter than 250 ms to obtain the preprocessed time series, (b) extract the distribution of percept phase durations for each state from the processed time series, and (c) calculate the median and sum of these distributions to obtain the median and overall fraction of each of the states in each rivalry block. 
Using this paradigm, we computed median phase durations as well as overall fractions for each of our five percept states (i.e., left, right, balanced mixed, mixed left, and mixed right; Figure 2B), allowing us to calculate ratios between the median phase durations of exclusive percepts (exclusive left vs. exclusive right) and mixed percepts (mixed left vs. mixed right). Although mean rivalry phase durations are used commonly in the literature to quantify perceptual dominance during rivalry (Blake & Logothetis, 2002; Klink et al., 2010; Lunghi et al., 2011; Sheynin et al., 2019; Zhou, Gao, White, Merk, & Yao, 2004), calculating the mean of the distribution is prone to be biased in favor of longer phase durations (Zhou et al., 2004); therefore, to account for this, we used the median rather than the mean of the phase duration distribution as a measure of perceptual dominance for each category. 
Figure 2
 
Partitioning original rivalry data into different dependent variables. (A) Observer's rivalry percept. (B) Ideal observer's key press response corresponding to percept. (C) Obtaining phase durations of overall mixed visibility We concatenated adjacent mixed percepts reported using the three mixed states in the original task to compute a new aggregated mixed percept state from which we extracted the median duration of mixed visibility.
Figure 2
 
Partitioning original rivalry data into different dependent variables. (A) Observer's rivalry percept. (B) Ideal observer's key press response corresponding to percept. (C) Obtaining phase durations of overall mixed visibility We concatenated adjacent mixed percepts reported using the three mixed states in the original task to compute a new aggregated mixed percept state from which we extracted the median duration of mixed visibility.
In addition, our main measure of perceptual eye dominance was defined by  
\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\(\def\bupalpha{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C2}}\)\(\def\bupbeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C3}}\)\(\def\bupgamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C4}}\)\(\def\bupdelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C5}}\)\(\def\bupepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C6}}\)\(\def\bupvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DC}}\)\(\def\bupzeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C7}}\)\(\def\bupeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C8}}\)\(\def\buptheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C9}}\)\(\def\bupiota{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CA}}\)\(\def\bupkappa{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CB}}\)\(\def\buplambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CC}}\)\(\def\bupmu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CD}}\)\(\def\bupnu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CE}}\)\(\def\bupxi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CF}}\)\(\def\bupomicron{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D0}}\)\(\def\buppi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D1}}\)\(\def\buprho{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D2}}\)\(\def\bupsigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D4}}\)\(\def\buptau{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D5}}\)\(\def\bupupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D6}}\)\(\def\bupphi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D7}}\)\(\def\bupchi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D8}}\)\(\def\buppsy{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D9}}\)\(\def\bupomega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DA}}\)\(\def\bupvartheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DD}}\)\(\def\bGamma{\bf{\Gamma}}\)\(\def\bDelta{\bf{\Delta}}\)\(\def\bTheta{\bf{\Theta}}\)\(\def\bLambda{\bf{\Lambda}}\)\(\def\bXi{\bf{\Xi}}\)\(\def\bPi{\bf{\Pi}}\)\(\def\bSigma{\bf{\Sigma}}\)\(\def\bUpsilon{\bf{\Upsilon}}\)\(\def\bPhi{\bf{\Phi}}\)\(\def\bPsi{\bf{\Psi}}\)\(\def\bOmega{\bf{\Omega}}\)\(\def\iGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E4}}\)\(\def\iDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E5}}\)\(\def\iTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E9}}\)\(\def\iLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EC}}\)\(\def\iXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EF}}\)\(\def\iPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F1}}\)\(\def\iSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F4}}\)\(\def\iUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F6}}\)\(\def\iPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F7}}\)\(\def\iPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F9}}\)\(\def\iOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6FA}}\)\(\def\biGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D71E}}\)\(\def\biDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D71F}}\)\(\def\biTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D723}}\)\(\def\biLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D726}}\)\(\def\biXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D729}}\)\(\def\biPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D72B}}\)\(\def\biSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D72E}}\)\(\def\biUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D730}}\)\(\def\biPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D731}}\)\(\def\biPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D733}}\)\(\def\biOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D734}}\)\begin{equation}\tag{1}{\rm{ODI}} = \left( {{{{d_{{\rm{non {\mbox{-}} deprived}}}} - {d_{{\rm{deprived}}}}} \over {{d_{{\rm{non {\mbox{-}} deprived}}}} + {d_{{\rm{deprived}}}}}}} \right),\end{equation}
where the two d variables are the overall fractions for the exclusive percepts from the nondeprived and deprived eyes. This ratio computed a value between −1 and 1, the extreme values indicating completely monocular vision from the nondeprived and deprived eyes, respectively. To evaluate deprivation-induced changes in these indices, we subtracted the baseline ratio from each postpatching ODI measure.  
Importantly, our 5AFC design allowed us to repartition the three intermediate mixed percepts into a single variable: mixed visibility. This was achieved by concatenating adjacent mixed percepts in the original rivalry time series data (i.e., mixed left + balanced mixed + mixed right) to obtain a single mixed percept state. We then administered our preprocessing paradigm on this repartitioned time series to obtain distributions of phase durations for three percept states: exclusive left eye, mixed, and exclusive right eye (Figure 2C). We used the distribution corresponding to the repartitioned “mixed” category to calculate the overall fraction and median duration of mixed visibility. 
To assess patching-induced effects across subjects and to account for intersubject variability at baseline, we calculated a value that represented the magnitude of the effect of patching on each dependent variable for each individual with respect to baseline. These values were obtained by dividing postpatching measures by those at baseline and then subtracting the normalized baseline. We conducted null hypothesis pairwise t tests on these normalized post/baseline values that determined whether deprivation significantly shifted the mean with respect to baseline (zero). We used the initial postdeprivation value for each dependent variable under the a priori assumption that the effect was maximal immediately after removing the patch. P values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR) correction method outlined in Benajmini and Hochberg (1995). We obtained 95% confidence intervals and the standard deviation of a distribution of 1,000 bootstrapped resamples (each drawing 13 subjects with replacement) of the normalized post/baseline values for each dependent variable. All SEMs in the current paper are equivalent to the standard deviation of the respective bootstrap distribution. 
Further, we also conducted a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA on the postdeprivation measures from the subset of observers who completed additional rivalry blocks over the course of an hour after patching. This analysis, administered on normalized post/baseline values at 0, 30, and 60 min after patching, was used to establish the time course of the decay of the effects of patching. We compared the normalized post/baseline values across the three measured time points to determine the time course of the decay and then administered post hoc t tests to determine which time points were significantly shifted with respect to baseline. 
Finally, we implemented a principal component analysis (PCA) to analyze the median duration data drawn from the reduced rivalry time series illustrated in Figure 2C. PCA is a statistical procedure that uses an orthogonal transformation to convert a set of observations of possibly correlated variables into a set of values of uncorrelated variables called principal components (PCs). We observed that the median durations of the two exclusive percepts at baseline were highly correlated with one another (Spearman rho = 0.93, p < 0.0001); therefore, a PCA transformation of the data would assist in mining statistically uncorrelated latent variables from the data that are arguably more informative of the neural processes underlying rivalry than the original task response variables used for analysis (Reynaud & Hess, 2017). 
We used MATLAB's built-in PCA function, specifying a singular value decomposition algorithm to extract the 3 × 3 coefficient matrix of three PCs (three PCs explain 100% of the variance in a 3-D data set) from the baseline median duration data. We then used this coefficient matrix to project both the baseline and postpatching median duration data into the PC space defined at baseline using the procedure  
\begin{equation}\tag{2}{{\bf{A}}_i} = {{\bf{X}}_i}\cdot {\bf{C}},\!\end{equation}
where Ai is the representation of median duration data Xi at time point i (baseline or postpatching) in the PC space defined at baseline by the PC coefficient matrix C. Both Ai and Xi are N × 3 matrices, where N represents the total number of participants. The columns of Xi correspond to the median durations of the three percept categories (exclusive left, mixed, exlcusive right), and the columns of Ai correspond to the PC scores for the three PCs extracted at baseline defined by coefficient matrix C. We then conducted FDR-corrected pair-wise t tests on the postbaseline values for each PC column j in Ai (i.e., A2jA1j) to evaluate patching-induced changes in the relative weight of each component's influence on binocular rivalry dynamics with respect to baseline. Importantly, PCA does not rely on our a priori assumptions (and subsequent dependent variables of interest) of the underlying processes driving rivalry phase durations. On the contrary, PCA uncovers statistically uncorrelated components of rivalry phase-duration data that may then map onto our understanding of the neural mechanisms involved in binocular rivalry, allowing us to evaluate patching-induced changes within those components.  
Results
We first analyzed the processed rivalry time series data to obtain median phase durations and overall fractions for each of our five percept states. We were interested to see how patching affected the fractions (Figure 3A) and median phase durations (Figure 3B) for the original five percept categories. 
Figure 3
 
Patching-induced changes in overall fractions and median phase durations. From the top down, the five percept states are (a) exclusive percepts from the deprived eye, (b) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the deprived eye, (c) the balanced mixed percepts, (d) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the nondeprived eye, and (e) the exclusive percept from the nondeprived eye. (A) Overall fractions. The left column shows individual participants' baseline fraction durations for each percept plotted against their postdeprivation fraction durations; the right column illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation (with replacement) on the pooled normalized postbaseline values for each percept category. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline values. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (B) Median phase durations. See panel A for corresponding information. N = 13; *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < .001.
Figure 3
 
Patching-induced changes in overall fractions and median phase durations. From the top down, the five percept states are (a) exclusive percepts from the deprived eye, (b) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the deprived eye, (c) the balanced mixed percepts, (d) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the nondeprived eye, and (e) the exclusive percept from the nondeprived eye. (A) Overall fractions. The left column shows individual participants' baseline fraction durations for each percept plotted against their postdeprivation fraction durations; the right column illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation (with replacement) on the pooled normalized postbaseline values for each percept category. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline values. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (B) Median phase durations. See panel A for corresponding information. N = 13; *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < .001.
In contrast with previous findings (Lunghi et al., 2011), our results indicate that neither the fraction nor median duration of the deprived eye's exclusive percept increase significantly after deprivation (fraction: M = 0.03, 95% CI: [−0.10, 0.16], FDR-corrected p > 0.05; median duration: M = 0.06, 95% CI: [−0.05, 0.18], FDR-corrected p > 0.05). However, we do find that the fraction and median duration of the exclusive percept of the nondeprived eye decrease significantly: fraction, M = −0.31, 95% CI: [−0.41, −0.20], t(12) = −5.41, FDR-corrected p < 0.001; median duration, M = −0.15, 95% CI: [−0.24, −0.04], t(12) = −2.91, FDR-corrected p < 0.05. This implies that the shift in perceptual eye dominance observed after patching may be driven by a decrease in the input strength of the nondeprived eye's image rather than a reciprocal increase in the deprived eye's contribution. 
Further, the median duration of the mixed percepts biased in favor of the nondeprived eye's image increased significantly after patching, mean difference = 0.30, 95% CI: [0.17, 0.46], t(12) = −4.09, FDR-corrected p < 0.01, as did that of the deprived eye's image, mean difference = 0.28, 95% CI: [0.09, 0.51], FDR-corrected p > 0.05. Increases in the overall fractions of all three mixed percepts were also observed: fraction mixed (nondeprived eye), M = 0.46, 95% CI: [0.15, 0.89], t(12) = 3.19, FDR-corrected p < 0.05; fraction mixed (balanced), M = 0.72, 95% CI: [0.11, 1.55], FDR-corrected p > 0.05; fraction mixed (deprived eye), M = 0.47, 95% CI: [0.21, 0.75], t(12) = 3.19, FDR-corrected p < 0.05. These results indicate that the mixed percepts were enhanced without the introduction of eye-specific bias. 
To further investigate, we analyzed changes in the overall fraction (Figure 4A) and median duration (Figure 4B) of overall mixed visibility (extracted from the reduced time series illustrated in Figure 2C). Patching significantly increased both the overall fraction of mixed visibility, Figure 4A, M = 0.33, bootstrapped 95% CI: [0.19, 0.52], t(12) = 3.51, FDR-corrected p < 0.01, and the median duration of mixed visibility, Figure 4B, M = 0.30, bootstrapped 95% CI: [0.17, 0.44], t(12) = 4.17, FDR-corrected p < 0.01. The shift in perceptual eye dominance (using the exclusive percepts) was also highly significant, M = 0.20, 95% CI: [0.11, 0.29], t(12) = 4.42, p < 0.001, Figure 4C. Interestingly, we did not observe a significant shift in perceptual eye dominance within the mixed percepts, mean difference = 0.03, 95% CI: [−0.04, 0.11], t(13) = 0.78, p > 0.05, further suggesting that the shift in perceptual eye dominance and the increase in mixed visibility may be separate effects of patching. 
Figure 4
 
Patching-induced changes in mixed visibility and perceptual eye dominance (A) Normalized postbaseline overall fraction of mixed visibility. Scatterplot (left) illustrating individual subjects' baseline fraction of mixed visibility (N = 13), plotted against their initial postdeprivation fraction of mixed visibility; middle panel illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation on the postbaseline differences. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline differences; right panel demonstrates individual normalized postbaseline overall fractions of mixed visibility at 0, 30, and 60 min after deprivation. The gray markers indicate the group mean. n = 5 (three women, age 24 ± 2.1). (B) Normalized postbaseline overall median duration mixed visibility. See panel A for corresponding information. (C) Normalized postbaseline perceptual eye dominance over the course of 1 hr after deprivation. Positive values indicate shifted bias in favor of the deprived eye. The perceptual eye ODI used to calculate these means utilized the median duration of the exclusive percepts from the deprived and nondeprived eyes. See panel A for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate means significantly shifted with respect to baseline. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < 0.001.
Figure 4
 
Patching-induced changes in mixed visibility and perceptual eye dominance (A) Normalized postbaseline overall fraction of mixed visibility. Scatterplot (left) illustrating individual subjects' baseline fraction of mixed visibility (N = 13), plotted against their initial postdeprivation fraction of mixed visibility; middle panel illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation on the postbaseline differences. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline differences; right panel demonstrates individual normalized postbaseline overall fractions of mixed visibility at 0, 30, and 60 min after deprivation. The gray markers indicate the group mean. n = 5 (three women, age 24 ± 2.1). (B) Normalized postbaseline overall median duration mixed visibility. See panel A for corresponding information. (C) Normalized postbaseline perceptual eye dominance over the course of 1 hr after deprivation. Positive values indicate shifted bias in favor of the deprived eye. The perceptual eye ODI used to calculate these means utilized the median duration of the exclusive percepts from the deprived and nondeprived eyes. See panel A for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate means significantly shifted with respect to baseline. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < 0.001.
For five out of the 13 participants, we collected data from rivalry blocks at 0, 30, and 60 min after patching to determine the time course of the decay of the patching-induced effects. We conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs on the group means for the three postdeprivation measurements to evaluate whether the patching-induced shifts changed significantly over the course of the experiment. Due to the small number of participants in this subset, most of our statistical tests for these analyses were underpowered. They still, however, give a noteworthy insight into both the intersubject variability of these effects and their time courses over an hour after patching. 
The ANOVA did not yield a significant decay of the effect of MD on the overall fraction of mixed visibility (which was weak to begin with in this smaller subsample), Wilks' lambda = 0.60, F(2, 10) = 1.30, p < 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.21, (Figure 4A, right panel). However, there was an observable trend of recovery to baseline levels over the course of an hour after patching (t0: M = 0.74, 95% CI: [−0.22, 1.71]; t30: M = 0.65, 95% CI: [0.06, 1.23]; t60: M = 0.31, 95% CI: [−0.37, 0.99]). 
Likewise, the ANOVA did not produce a significant decay of the effect of MD on the median duration of mixed visibility, Wilks' lambda = 0.60, F(2, 8) = 1.55, p > 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.28 (Figure 4B); however, there was also an observable trend of recovery to baseline levels over the course of an hour after patching (t0: M = 1.18, 95% CI: [0.06, 2.30]; t30: M = 0.64, 95% CI: [−0.43, 1.74]; t60: M = 0.28, 95% CI: [−0.55, 1.13]). 
Finally, the decay of the effect of MD on perceptual eye dominance was also not significant for this subset of participants, Wilks' lambda = 0.27, F(2, 8) = 1.78, p > 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.30 (Figure 4C, right panel). However, perceptual eye dominance was significantly shifted with respect to baseline immediately following MD, M = 0.12, 95% CI: [0.10, 0.23], t(4) = 3.1, FDR-corrected p < 0.05), as well at 30 min after removing the patch, M = 0.08, 95% CI: [0.05, 0.11], t(4) = 7.37, FDR-corrected p < 0.01, but not at 60 min (FDR-corrected p > 0.05), suggesting a gradual recovery to baseline levels. 
Our initial analyses found that the median durations of the two exclusive percepts are highly correlated with one another (Spearman rho = 0.93, p < 0.001). This finding inspired us to utilize a PCA to transform the variables in our median duration data set (exclusive left, mixed, exclusive right) into a new set of statistically uncorrelated variables that were possibly more informative of neural processes underlying rivalry. We administered a descriptive PCA on the baseline median durations extracted from the processed time series illustrated in Figure 2C to uncover three PCs that explained 100% of the variability in our data (Figure 5A). The PCA coefficients indicate the degree to which each PC (PCs 1–3) is associated with the original rivalry percept variables. PC 1 is most closely associated with the median duration of mixed visibility and explains 70.10% of the variability in the baseline data. For the purpose of this analysis, PC 1 can be interpreted as the binocular combination component underlying rivalry phase durations. For PC 2, the PCA extracted the correlation between the two exclusive percept variables; PC 2 is most closely associated with the median duration of both exclusive percepts and explains 28.94% of variability in the data. PC 2 can then be feasibly regarded as the perceptual suppression component underlying rivalry phase durations. Finally, PC 3 is anticorrelated between the two exclusive percepts and uncorrelated with mixed visibility; this PC explains the remaining 0.95% of the variability in the data. PC 3 points to interocular balance, or perceptual eye dominance, as a small underlying component influencing the baseline rivalry phase duration data. 
Figure 5
 
PCA on median rivalry phase duration data. (A) Output of the PCA. The PCA was administered on baseline rivalry phase durations drawn from the reduced processed time series illustrated in Figure 2C. The components are statistically uncorrelated, pointing to three unique processes underlying the phase-duration data. The PCA coefficients indicate the degree to which each principal component (PCs 1–3) is associated with the median durations of each percept type. PC 1 is most closely associated with the median duration of mixed visibility, PC 2 is most closely associated with the median duration of complete perceptual suppression, and PC 3 is plausibly interpreted as ocular imbalance or perceptual eye dominance (see Methods for more information on the PCA). (B) Correlating baseline PCA scores with baseline binocular rivalry features. The x-axis corresponds to the z-normalized PC scores for each PC across subjects; the y-axis values indicate z-normalized values corresponding to the following baseline median phase duration data: PC 1, median duration of mixed visibility; PC 2, median duration of exclusive visibility (the arithmetic mean of the exclusive percepts' median durations); and PC 3, the ratio of the median durations of the exclusive percepts defined in Equation 1. PC scores are highly correlated with their respective binocular rivalry features. (C) Comparing postbaseline PC scores. Pre- and postpatching PC scores were obtained using the method outlined in Methods. PC scores indicate the degree to which each PC weighs on an individual's rivalry data. Each bar indicates the group M ± SEM. See panel C for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate significant interactions. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05.
Figure 5
 
PCA on median rivalry phase duration data. (A) Output of the PCA. The PCA was administered on baseline rivalry phase durations drawn from the reduced processed time series illustrated in Figure 2C. The components are statistically uncorrelated, pointing to three unique processes underlying the phase-duration data. The PCA coefficients indicate the degree to which each principal component (PCs 1–3) is associated with the median durations of each percept type. PC 1 is most closely associated with the median duration of mixed visibility, PC 2 is most closely associated with the median duration of complete perceptual suppression, and PC 3 is plausibly interpreted as ocular imbalance or perceptual eye dominance (see Methods for more information on the PCA). (B) Correlating baseline PCA scores with baseline binocular rivalry features. The x-axis corresponds to the z-normalized PC scores for each PC across subjects; the y-axis values indicate z-normalized values corresponding to the following baseline median phase duration data: PC 1, median duration of mixed visibility; PC 2, median duration of exclusive visibility (the arithmetic mean of the exclusive percepts' median durations); and PC 3, the ratio of the median durations of the exclusive percepts defined in Equation 1. PC scores are highly correlated with their respective binocular rivalry features. (C) Comparing postbaseline PC scores. Pre- and postpatching PC scores were obtained using the method outlined in Methods. PC scores indicate the degree to which each PC weighs on an individual's rivalry data. Each bar indicates the group M ± SEM. See panel C for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate significant interactions. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05.
We transformed both the baseline median duration data and the postpatching median duration data by projecting these data sets into the PC space defined by the coefficient matrix extracted from the baseline data. This procedure yielded two data sets, corresponding to the PC scores for each participant for each PC before and after monocular patching (see “Methods” for more details). As a sanity check, we confirmed correlations between these PC scores and the features they represented in the baseline data. For PC 1, this was the median duration of mixed visibility; for PC 2, this was the median duration of exclusive visibility (the arithmetic mean of the median durations of the two exclusive percepts); and for PC 3, this was perceptual eye dominance, calculated using the procedure outlined in Equation 1. We z-normalized (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) both the PC scores and their corresponding features in the original data set to ensure both sets were scaled similarly for comparison. The PC scores were all significantly correlated with the features we extracted from the original data set, Fs(1, 12) ≥ 21.4, ps < 0.001, adjusted R2 ≥ 0.61, indicating the PCA successfully extracted meaningful components underlying the phase-duration data at baseline (Figure 5C). 
FDR-corrected pair-wise t tests were conducted on the postbaseline PC scores. We found that patching significantly increased the mean score of PC 1, M = 0.65, 95% CI: [0.09, 1.21], t(13) = 2.52, FDR-corrected p < 0.05, and PC 3, M = 0.33, 95% CI: [0.11, 0.55], t(13) = 3.31, FDR-corrected p < 0.05, but not PC 2, M = −0.06, 95% CI: [−0.44, 0.30], t(13) = −0.40; FDR-corrected p > 0.05. Notably, the PCA uncovered statistically uncorrelated components of rivalry phase duration data that map on quite well to our understanding of several factors involved in binocular rivalry: binocular combination, perceptual suppression, and perceptual eye dominance. This approach allowed us to evaluate patching-induced changes within these mechanistic components, extending the insights of the previous analyses. Specifically, our results indicate that MD affects putative neural mechanisms responsible for binocular combination and perceptual eye dominance rather than those responsible for exclusive dominance. 
Experiment 2
We designed Experiment 2 to investigate whether short-term monocular patching preferentially affects superimposition versus piecemeal mixed percepts during binocular rivalry. This experiment used a 4AFC binocular rivalry task adapted from Skerswetat et al. (2017) to evaluate patching-induced changes in rivalry dynamics. 
Methods and materials
Observers
A total of 11 individuals enrolled in Experiment 2 (eight women, 21, ± 2.1, one author). One participant was excluded from the study due to a failure to complete the full experiment; therefore, in sum, 10 individuals participated the study. Two participants completed both Experiments 1 and 2
Apparatus
Each session took place in a quiet room with dim light. The original display system we used in Experiment 1 was not available at the time of data collection for Experiment 2; therefore, stimuli were displayed on the Oculus DK2 VR headset to dichoptically present the binocular rivalry stimuli generated and controlled by the same computer system as described in Experiment 1. The Oculus was gamma-corrected with a mean luminance of 90 cd/m2, driven at a resolution of 960 × 1,080 per eye, with a refresh rate of 60 Hz and a nominal field of view of 100°. The left- and right-eye images were separated by a divider such that the left eye only viewed the left side of the goggles and the right eye only viewed the right side. 
Stimulus
The dichoptic stimulus used in Experiment 2 was identical to that of Experiment 1 with the exception that we used a larger stimulus (4 c/°, subtending a diameter of 2° with a raised cosine annulus blurring the edges, Michelson contrast = 80%) due to the pixel density limitations of the Oculus DK2 headset. 
Binocular rivalry task and experimental protocol
We adapted a 4AFC binocular rivalry task used by Skerswetat et al. (2017; Figure 1D) to quantify the overall fraction of exclusive, piecemeal, and superimposition mixed percepts. At the beginning of each session, participants were shown images on a document that illustrated the differences between the left-oriented, right-oriented, and superimposition versus piecemeal mixed percepts. Participants were told that they would see a dynamic stimulus during the experiment and that their task was to track what they were seeing with particular attention to timeliness and accuracy. Aside from the response criteria, all other aspects of the stimulus and task were identical to that described for Experiment 1
Participants were given the option to continuously indicate whether they were seeing either (a) an exclusively left-tilted grating, (b) an exclusively right-tilted grating, (c) a superimposition mixed percept, or (d) a piecemeal mixed percept. Participants used three adjacent keys for the task, using the left to indicate exclusive left tilt, right for right tilt, holding down a combination of the left and right keys for the piecemeal percepts, and the middle key for the superimposition percepts. In our instructions, we specified that exclusive percepts were those with 90% or more left- or right-tilted lines, and the mixed percepts were between 50% and 90% left- or right-tilted lines. Postdeprivation assessments were administered at 0, 15, 30, and 60 min after patching. 
Preprocessing and Statistical Analysis
We used the same preprocessing paradigm described in Experiment 1; however, we also developed an additional dependent variable to investigate postbaseline differences in the mixed percept ratio (MPR) defined by the following equation:  
\begin{equation}\tag{3}{\rm{MPR}} = \left( {{{{d_{{\rm{superimposition}}}} - {d_{{\rm{piecemeal}}}}} \over {{d_{{\rm{superimposition}}}} + {d_{{\rm{piecemeal}}}}}}} \right),\end{equation}
where the two d variables indicate the overall fraction reported for seeing superimposition and piecemeal percepts, respectively. Negative values in the MPR indicated bias in favor of superimposition percepts, and positive values indicated bias in favor of piecemeal percepts. Patching-induced changes in the MPR were obtained by subtracting baseline from postpatching values.  
We conducted pairwise t tests on the first postpatching measurement and baseline for (a) the overall fraction of mixed visibility, (b) the median duration of mixed visibility, (c) the mixed percept ratio (MPR), and (d) the eye dominance index (ODI). We also conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA with complementary post hoc paired t tests on the MPR and ODI values to determine the time course of the decay of the effect of patching on these variables. 
Results
We first wanted to see if the results observed in Experiment 1 were also measurable using a binocular rivalry task with different response instructions. Using this rivalry task, we replicated the finding that 2 hr of MD increases both the fraction, M = 1.09, 95% CI: [0.24, 1.93], t(9) = 2.98, p < 0.05, and median duration, M = 0.24, 95% CI: [0.04, 0.43], t(9) = 2.74, p < 0.05, of mixed visibility during rivalry. Perceptual eye dominance was also significantly shifted in favor of the deprived eye with respect to baseline, M = 0.12, 95% CI: [0.01, 0.23], t(9) = 2.52, p < 0.05. 
Furthermore, we also found that the MPR shifts significantly in favor of superimposition immediately after MD, M = −0.25, 95% CI: [−0.62, −0.12], t(9) = −2.75, p < 0.05. This indicates that the increase in mixed visibility observed in this experiment and in Experiment 1 is likely due to increases in the superimposition percepts rather than piecemeal percepts. This was confirmed by separate paired t tests on the normalized postbaseline fractions for both superimposition and piecemeal percepts immediately after deprivation: superimposition, M = 0.08, 95% CI: [0.03, 0.12], t(9) = 4.01, FDR-corrected p < 0.01; piecemeal, M = −0.02, 95% CI: [−0.07, 0.02], t(9) = −1.06, FDR-corrected p > 0.05. 
To determine the time course of this effect of deprivation, we also conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs on the change in the overall fraction of mixed visibility, the shift in the MPR (Figure 6A), and the shift in perceptual eye dominance (Figure 6B) across four postdeprivation time points at 0, 15, 30, and 60 min after removing the patch. 
Figure 6
 
Experiment 2: Patching-induced changes in superimposition versus piecemeal mixed visibility during rivalry. (A) Decay of patching-induced effect on fraction mixed visibility and MPR. The overall fraction of mixed visibility (red + green) is the sum of the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts (shown in green) and that of the piecemeal percepts (shown in red). The top rows of asterisked interactions indicate a significant increase in the fraction of mixed visibility at t0 and t15 with respect to baseline. The bottom row of asterisked interactions indicates an increase in the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts at t0, t15, and t60 with respect to baseline. Piecemeal percepts did not shift significantly with respect to baseline. (B) Decay of patching-induced effect on perceptual eye dominance. Perceptual eye dominance gradually recovers to baseline. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to baseline. *p < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
Experiment 2: Patching-induced changes in superimposition versus piecemeal mixed visibility during rivalry. (A) Decay of patching-induced effect on fraction mixed visibility and MPR. The overall fraction of mixed visibility (red + green) is the sum of the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts (shown in green) and that of the piecemeal percepts (shown in red). The top rows of asterisked interactions indicate a significant increase in the fraction of mixed visibility at t0 and t15 with respect to baseline. The bottom row of asterisked interactions indicates an increase in the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts at t0, t15, and t60 with respect to baseline. Piecemeal percepts did not shift significantly with respect to baseline. (B) Decay of patching-induced effect on perceptual eye dominance. Perceptual eye dominance gradually recovers to baseline. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to baseline. *p < 0.05.
The decay of the effect of MD on perceptual eye dominance across these four time points was not significant for these observers, Wilks' lambda = 0.47, F(3, 27) = 0.38, p > 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.13 (Figure 6B). However, the perceptual eye dominance shift was significant immediately after removing the eye patch and remained significant until 30 min after removing the patch, t(9) > 2.98, FDR-corrected ps < 0.05. 
Likewise, the decay of the effect of MD on the overall fraction of mixed visibility across the five time points was also not significant, Wilks' lambda = 0.39, F(3, 27) = 1.30, p > 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.13 (Figure 6A). However, the magnitude of the effect of MD on the fraction of mixed visibility was greatest directly following MD, M = 0.31, 95% CI: [0.06, 0.56], t(9) = 2.8, FDR-corrected p < 0.05, as well as at 15 min after removing the patch, M = 0.30, 95% CI: [0.15, 0.45], t(9) = 4.65, FDR-corrected p < 0.01. 
Similarly, the decay effect of the effect of MD on the MPR was also not significant, Wilks' lambda = 0.63, F(3, 27) = 0.70, p > 0.05, Display Formula\(\eta _p^2\) = 0.07 (Figure 6A), suggesting the ratio of superimposition to piecemeal percepts did not change significantly over the course of our postdeprivation measurements. The MPR did, however, shift significantly in favor of superimposition with respect to baseline across three out of four of our measure time points: 0, 15, and up to at least 60 min after MD, t(9) > 2.98, FDR-corrected ps < 0.05. 
Discussion
We conducted two experiments to characterize the effects of short-term monocular patching on the occurrence of mixed percepts during binocular rivalry. Our investigation was inspired by recent findings that patching alters E-I balance in visual cortex (Binda et al., 2017; Chadnova et al., 2017; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015) and that the absolute predominance of mixed visibility during rivalry can be modified through recent visual experience (Klink et al., 2010; Said & Heeger, 2013) as well as with neuromodulators (Mentch et al., 2018). 
Experiment 1 utilized a rivalry task that enabled us to accurately quantify patching-induced changes in perceptual eye dominance as well as in the overall fraction and median duration of mixed visibility during rivalry. Our results from this experiment demonstrated that patching causes enhancements in both the fraction and median duration of mixed visibility during rivalry. Further, our data also suggest that patching achieves a perceptual eye dominance shift in favor of the deprived eye by reducing the overall predominance and median duration of the nondeprived eye's image while simultaneously reallocating its responses among the mixed percepts. 
This finding contrasts with previous rivalry studies on patching that found the shift in perceptual dominance is caused by an increase in the strength of the deprived eye and a reciprocal decrease in the nondeprived eye (Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2019; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015). It is important to note, however, that the conclusions drawn from our data are not entirely in disagreement with these previous results because the previous studies monocularly deprived the dominant eye and our study always deprived the nondominant eye. This distinction is possibly related to Levelt's proposition II (Levelt, 1965) or, more appropriately, modified proposition II (Brascamp, Klink, & Levelt, 2015), which states that, when the input strength of the two eyes are independently altered, the dominance duration of the eye with the stronger input is maximally affected. In the context of our study, it is reasonable to consider the nondeprived (dominant) eye as the eye with the stronger input at baseline. It is, therefore, plausible that patching-induced changes in the signal strength of the deprived eye would preferentially affect the dominance duration of the nondeprived eye as we observe in our results. 
Our study also demonstrated that perceptual eye dominance shifts within the exclusive percepts while the two biased mixed-percept categories increase independently of eye of origin. This finding presents the possibility that deprivation impacts exclusive dominance and mixtures differently. It is possible, however, that our participants did not accurately classify the three fractional mixed percept categories because they alternated faster and were more difficult to keep track of than the exclusive percepts. Although our task design sought to ensure that participants do not miscategorize “biased” mixed percepts as exclusive percepts, it also introduced a possible source of error in the categorization of the three mixed-percept categories. For this reason, it may be fruitful for future studies utilizing our task to employ a “replay” rivalry control condition to evaluate possible criterion effects latent in the task. Relatedly, an unpublished experiment conducted by the authors demonstrated that 5 min of monocular deprivation did not change the response criterion for mixed visibility during replay rivalry (although it did shift perceptual eye dominance and also enhance mixed visibility in normal rivalry). Our approach to circumnavigate this issue in the present study, however, was to implement a PCA that aimed to transform the original data into statistically uncorrelated components underlying rivalry phase durations that were invariant to eye of origin. Using this approach, we identified three components that corresponded to several hypothesized mechanisms involved in producing the rivalry states: (a) binocular combination, (b) perceptual suppression (agnostic to eye of origin), and (c) perceptual eye dominance. This approach, which has previously been used to infer neural mechanisms from behavioral data (Reynaud & Hess, 2017), offered additional evidence for the idea that MD has two identifiable and statistically distinguishable effects on binocular rivalry dynamics: (a) an increase in ocular imbalance and (b) an increase in binocular combination. 
The main conclusions drawn from Experiment 1 can be plausibly understood by the idea that MD achieves its effects by weakening interocular inhibition. We designed Experiment 2 to assess this idea. We adapted a rivalry task previously developed by Skerswetat et al. (2017) to investigate patching-induced changes in the relative predominance of superimposition and piecemeal mixed percepts. Whereas superimposition percepts can be thought of as fully fused binocular percepts, the result of weakened interocular suppression, piecemeal percepts can be considered to be intermediary binocular percepts, in which rivalry is still occurring in smaller subregions (Alais & Melcher, 2007; Klink et al., 2010; Kovacs et al., 1996; Lee & Blake, 2004). Superimposition and piecemeal mixed percepts have been previously attributed to arise from two different but related aspects of interocular inhibition: gain and spatial coherence, respectively (Klink et al., 2010). Superimposition percepts would then indicate a reduction in the overall gain of interocular inhibition while picemeal perception points to reduced spatial coherence of interocular inhibition. The finding that patching enhances the relative predominance of superimposition percepts, although not significantly affecting piecemeal visibility, adds complementary evidence to the idea that MD attenuates the gain of interocular inhibitory interactions. 
It is also noteworthy to add that superimposition percepts during binocular rivalry are known to appear infrequently with the stimulus parameters used in our study (Hollins, 1980), and when they are visible, the component gratings often do not appear equal in clarity and contrast (Yang, Rose, & Blake, 1992). Assuming the neural mechanisms underlying superimposed visibility immediately after patching are the same ones promoting those states at baseline, the significant increase in superimposition visibility implicates patching as a potent method to reduce interocular inhibition. In this way, our results are related to previous investigations evaluating the role of inhibitory interocular interactions in rivalry, in which prolonged exposure to rivalrous binocular stimuli also causes an increase in superimposed mixed visibility (Klink et al., 2010; Said & Heeger, 2013). 
Although the effects of short-term patching on perceptual eye dominance are well documented (Baldwin & Hess, 2018; Kim et al., 2017; Lunghi, Berchicci, et al., 2015; Lunghi et al., 2011; Zhou, Clavagnier, & Hess, 2013), the current study presents the first evidence that monocular patching also enhances binocular combination. Previous studies using binocular rivalry with similar stimulus parameters to assess the effects of monocular patching (Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2019; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015) excluded participants with greater than 20% overall predominance of mixed visibility at baseline. Although such exclusion criteria may improve accuracy in measures of perceptual eye dominance, it also reduces the generalizability of these results to the overall population, in which the average proportion of mixed visibility using the stimulus parameters mentioned in our paper ranges between 30% and 60% (O'Shea et al., 1997). 
Theoretical implications
Importantly, our main findings are compatible with several proposed computational frameworks of binocular rivalry. For instance, the patching-induced increase in mixed visibility aligns well with the work done by Brascamp et al. (2013). In this paper, the authors present an experimentally derived model of rivalry in which eye-specific neural events in early processing areas contribute to perceptual competition during stimulus rivalry (in which incongruent images are continuously swapped between the two eyes but representations of the images rival as in binocular rivalry). As patching likely causes changes in early eye-specific cortical areas (Chadnova et al., 2017; Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015; Tso et al., 2017), our data contribute to the idea that changes in monocular neural activity can modulate the resolution of binocular rivalry. This model contrasts with other computational approaches to binocular rivalry that attribute perceptual competition to exclusively higher-order binocular areas (Wilson, 2003). An interesting avenue of future study will be to investigate whether patching also affects perceptual eye dominance and mixed visibility during stimulus rivalry. Such work can further reveal the neural loci of the two identifiable effects of monocular patching on rivalry dynamics mentioned in the current study. 
Likewise, the finding that patching enhances binocular combination is compatible with computational frameworks of rivalry that include opponency neurons (Blake, 1989; Li, Rankin, Rinzel, Carrasco, & Heeger, 2017; Said & Heeger, 2013). Opponency neurons, or XOR neurons, detect interocular conflict and play a role in the resolution of binocular rivalry. In the Said and Heeger (2013) model of binocular rivalry, opponency neurons inhibit preceding feed-forward units such that an adaptive reduction in the activity of these inhibitory interneurons results in a facilitation of binocular combination. This model succeeds at predicting experimental evidence in which adaptation to interocular flicker of left- and right-oriented monocular gratings (targeting opponency neurons) subsequently produces more mixed visibility during rivalry than a binocular adaptor of the same stimuli (not targeting opponency neurons). Similarly, in our case, temporarily depriving one eye of input can be conceived of as (a) preferentially adapting binocular opponency neurons and also (b) adapting the feed-forward monocular signal of the nondeprived eye. Removing the eye patch subsequently causes a relative enhancement in (a) the perception of mixtures and (b) a shift in balance in favor of the deprived eye. It is worth mentioning that recent physiological evidence has identified populations of neurons that are synchronized with the intermodulation of monocular SSVEP signals during rivalry (Katyal, Engel, He, & He, 2016) in line with these theoretical insights (Blake, 1989; Li et al., 2017; Said & Heeger, 2013). 
Finally, it is feasible to consider that the two effects of MD on binocular rivalry dynamics discussed in this article emerge, in part, as a result of the type of attentional gain mechanism described in Li et al. (2017). Attention is a well-established factor influencing binocular rivalry dynamics (see Carrasco, 2011; Dieter & Tadin, 2011; Dieter, Brascamp, Tadin, & Blake, 2016). In their model, Li et al. (2017) propose that attentional modulation from higher-order visual areas amplifies perceptual competition by biasing attentional gain to one of the rival stimuli. According to the model, prolonged adaptation of such an attentional mechanism would subsequently result in a decrease of perceptual suppression during rivalry. A patching-induced adaptation of this type of attentional mechanism could account for the reduction of perceptual exclusivity we observe in our experiments. Taking this possibility a step further, our findings may contribute new evidence for the existence of eye-specific attentional channels (Saban, Sekely, Klein, & Gabay, 2018; Self & Roelfsema, 2010), in which adaptation of the nondeprived eye's attentional channel subsequently shifts perceptual balance in favor of the deprived eye. 
Conclusion
In summary, our study provides new insights on the effects of short-term adult MD. Although we have known for some time that patching causes a temporary shift in perceptual eye dominance, we now know that some of this shift is attributed to a reallocation of responses toward the perception of mixtures. The findings of the present study contribute to the growing evidence that short-term MD causes a temporary functional plasticity observable at the level of E-I balance in early visual cortex. It will be beneficial for future rivalry studies on MD to take advantage of a detailed account of the intermediary mixed percepts to further advance our knowledge of the underlying brain mechanisms and to sharpen our understanding of binocular visual plasticity in general. 
Acknowledgments
We thank Randolph Blake for his comments on a previous version of this manuscript. This work was funded by an ERA-NET Neuron grant (JTC 2015) and Canadian Institutes of Health Research grants (CCI-125686 and 228103) awarded to RFH. Emoji icon made available for free at http://www.emojione.com
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Yasha Sheynin. 
Address: McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada. 
References
Alais, D., & Melcher, D. (2007). Strength and coherence of binocular rivalry depends on shared stimulus complexity. Vision Research, 47 (2), 269–279, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.09.003.
Baker, D. H., Kaestner, M., & Gouws, A. D. (2016). Measurement of crosstalk in stereoscopic display systems used for vision research. Journal of Vision, 16 (15): 14, 1–10, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.15.14. [PubMed] [Article]
Baldwin, A. S., & Hess, R. F. (2018). The mechanism of short-term monocular deprivation is not simple: Separate effects on parallel and cross-oriented dichoptic masking. Scientific Reports, 8 (1): 6191, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-24584-9.
Benajmini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57 (1), 289–300.
Binda, P., Kurzawski, J., Lunghi, C., Biagi, L., Tosetti, M., & Morrone, M. C. (2017). Short-term monocular deprivation enhances 7T BOLD responses and reduces neural selectivity in V1. Journal of Vision, 17 (10): 577, https://doi.org/10.1167/17.10.577. [Abstract]
Blake, R. (1989). A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychological Review, 96 (1), 145–167, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.145.
Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3 (1), 13–21, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn701.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10 (4), 433–436.
Brascamp, J. W., Klink, P. C., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2015). The ‘laws' of binocular rivalry: 50 years of Levelt's propositions. Vision Research, 109 (part A), 20–37, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.02.019.
Brascamp, J., Sohn, H., Lee, S.-H.,& Blake, R. (2013). A monocular contribution to stimulus rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 (21), 8337–8344, http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1305393110.
Brascamp, J. W., van Ee, R., Noest, A. J., Jacobs, R. H., & van den Berg, A. V. (2006). The time course of binocular rivalry reveals a fundamental role of noise. Journal of Vision, 6 (11): 8, 1244–1256, https://doi.org/10.1167/6.11.8. [PubMed] [Article]
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51 (13), 1484–1525, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012.
Chadnova, E., Reynaud, A., Clavagnier, S., & Hess, R. F. (2017). Short-term monocular occlusion produces changes in ocular dominance by a reciprocal modulation of interocular inhibition. Scientific Reports, 7, 41747, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41747.
Dieter, K. C., Brascamp, J., Tadin, D., & Blake, R. (2016). Does visual attention drive the dynamics of bistable perception? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 78 (7), 1861–1873, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1143-2.
Dieter, K. C., Sy, J. L., & Blake, R. (2016). Individual differences in sensory eye dominance reflected in the dynamics of binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 141 (2017), 40–50, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.014.
Dieter, K. C., & Tadin, D. (2011). Understanding attentional modulation of binocular rivalry: A framework based on biased competition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5 (December), 1–12, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00155.
Freyberg, J., Robertson, C. E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Reduced perceptual exclusivity during object and grating rivalry in autism. Journal of Vision, 15 (13): 11, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1167/15.13.11. [PubMed] [Article]
Hollins, M. (1980). The effect of contrast on the completeness of binocular rivalry suppression. Perception & Psychophysics, 27 (6), 550–556, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198684.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. The Journal of Physiology, 206 (2), 419–436, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022.
Katyal, S., Engel, S. A., He, B., & He, S. (2016). Neurons that detect interocular conflict during binocular rivalry revealed with EEG. Journal of Vision, 16 (3): 18, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1167/16.3.18. [PubMed] [Article]
Kim, H.-W., Kim, C.-Y., & Blake, R. (2017). Monocular perceptual deprivation from interocular suppression temporarily imbalances ocular dominance. Current Biology, 27 (6), 884–889, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.063.
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D.,& Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36 ECVP Abstract Supplement.
Klink, P. C., Brascamp, J. W., Blake, R., & Van Wezel, R. J. (2010). Experience-driven plasticity in binocular vision. Current Biology, 20 (16), 1464–1469, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057.
Kovacs, I., Papathomas, T. V., Yang, M., & Feher, A. (1996). When the brain changes its mind: Interocular grouping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 93 (26), 15508–15511.
Lee, S. H., & Blake, R. (2004). A fresh look at interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 44 (10), 983–991, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2003.12.007.
Levelt, W. (1965). On binocular rivalry. Soesterberg, The Netherlands: RVO–TNO, Institute for Perception, (pp. 1–107) https://doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.1578.
Li, H. H., Rankin, J., Rinzel, J., Carrasco, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2017). Attention model of binocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 114 (30), E6192–E6201, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620475114.
Liu, L., Tyler, C. W., & Schor, C. M. (1992). Failure of rivalry at low contrast: Evidence of a suprathreshold binocular summation process. Vision Research, 32 (8), 1471–1479, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(92)90203-U.
Lunghi, C., Berchicci, M., Morrone, M. C., & Di Russo, F. (2015). Short-term monocular deprivation alters early components of visual evoked potentials. The Journal of Physiology, 593 (19), 4361–4372, https://doi.org/10.1113/JP270950.
Lunghi, C., Burr, D. C., & Morrone, C. (2011). Brief periods of monocular deprivation disrupt ocular balance in human adult visual cortex. Current Biology, 21 (14), R538–R539, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.004.
Lunghi, C., Emir, U. E., Morrone, M. C., & Bridge, H. (2015). Short-term monocular deprivation alters GABA in the adult human visual cortex. Current Biology, 25 (11), 1496–1501, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.021.
Lunghi, C., Morrone, M. C., Secci, J., & Caputo, R. (2016). Binocular rivalry measured 2 hours after occlusion therapy predicts the recovery rate of the amblyopic eye in anisometropic children. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 57 (4), 1537–1546, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-18419.
Mentch, J., Spiegel, A., Ricciardi, C., Kanwisher, N., & Robertson, C. (2018). Causal push-and-pull modulation of binocular rivalry dynamics using GABAergic drugs. Journal of Vision, 18 (10): 956, https://doi.org/10.1167/18.10.956. [Abstract]
Miles, W. R. (1930) Ocular dominance in human adults. Journal of General Psychology, 4, 412–430.
O'Shea, R. P., Sims, A. J., & Govan, D. G. (1997). The effect of spatial frequency and field size on the spread of exclusive visibility in binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 37 (2), 175–183, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00113-7.
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10 (4), 437–442.
Reynaud, A., & Hess, R. F. (2017). Characterization of spatial frequency channels underlying disparity sensitivity by factor analysis of population data. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 11 (July), 1–6, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00063.
Saban, W., Sekely, L., Klein, R. M., & Gabay, S. (2018). Monocular channels have a functional role in endogenous orienting. Neuropsychologia, 111, 1–7, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.002.
Said, C. P., & Heeger, D. J. (2013). A model of binocular rivalry and cross-orientation suppression. PLoS Computational Biology, 9 (3):e1002991, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002991.
Self, M. W., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2010). A monocular, unconscious form of visual attention. Journal of Vision, 10 (4): 17, 1–22, https://doi.org/10.1167/10.4.17. [PubMed] [Article]
Sheynin, Y., Chamoun, M., Baldwin, A. S., Rosa-Neto, P., Hess, R. F., & Vaucher, E. (2019). Cholinergic potentiation alters perceptual eye dominance plasticity induced by a few hours of monocular patching in adults. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13 (January), 1–12, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00022.
Skerswetat, J., Formankiewicz, M. A., & Waugh, S. J. (2017). More superimposition for contrast-modulated than luminance-modulated stimuli during binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 142, 40–51, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2017.10.002.
Tso, D., Miller, R., & Begum, M. (2017). Neuronal responses underlying shifts in interocular balance induced by short-term deprivation in adult macaque visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 17 (10): 576, https://doi.org/10.1167/17.10.576. [Abstract]
Wilson, H. R. (2003). Computational evidence for a rivalry hierarchy in vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100 (24), 14499–14503, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2333622100.
Yang, Y., Rose, D., & Blake, R. (1992). On the variety of percepts associated with dichoptic viewing of dissimilar monocular stimuli. Perception, 21 (1), 47–62, https://doi.org/10.1068/p210047.
Zhou, J., Baker, D. H., Simard, M., Saint-Amour, D., & Hess, R. F. (2015). Short-term monocular patching boosts the patched eye's response in visual cortex. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 33 (3), 381–387, https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-140472.
Zhou, J., Clavagnier, S., & Hess, R. F. (2013). Short-term monocular deprivation strengthens the patched eye's contribution to binocular combination. Journal of Vision, 13 (5): 12, 1–10, https://doi.org/10.1167/13.5.12. [PubMed] [Article]
Zhou, Y. H., Gao, J. B., White, K. D., Merk, I., & Yao, K. (2004). Perceptual dominance time distributions in multistable visual perception. Biological Cybernetics, 90 (4), 256–263, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-004-0472-8.
Figure 1
 
Methods. (A) Experimental protocol. Baseline rivalry data was obtained from four 180-s rivalry blocks, each consisting of two 90-s rivalry runs. The first block of the baseline measurements was discarded. The baseline measurements were calculated by extracting the median of the three remaining blocks. Following baseline testing, we patched the participants' nondominant eye with a diffuser eye patch for 2 hr. After this, we continued with three postpatching rivalry blocks over the course of 9 min after removing the patch and extracted our main postpatching measurement by taking the median of these blocks. (B) Baseline data. Median phase durations (left) and overall fractions (right) (M ± SEM) for the five percept states obtained using the binocular rivalry task in Experiment 1. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (C) Experiment 1: 5AFC binocular rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (ML) a mostly left-oriented grating with some right-oriented lines, (M) a balanced left- and right-oriented grating (indicated by the absence of a key press), (MR) a mostly right-oriented grating with some left-oriented lines, or (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating. (D) Experiment 2: 4AFC superimposition versus piecemeal rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating, pressing both (L + R) simultaneously to indicate they were seeing a piecemeal percept, or (M) a superimposition percept.
Figure 1
 
Methods. (A) Experimental protocol. Baseline rivalry data was obtained from four 180-s rivalry blocks, each consisting of two 90-s rivalry runs. The first block of the baseline measurements was discarded. The baseline measurements were calculated by extracting the median of the three remaining blocks. Following baseline testing, we patched the participants' nondominant eye with a diffuser eye patch for 2 hr. After this, we continued with three postpatching rivalry blocks over the course of 9 min after removing the patch and extracted our main postpatching measurement by taking the median of these blocks. (B) Baseline data. Median phase durations (left) and overall fractions (right) (M ± SEM) for the five percept states obtained using the binocular rivalry task in Experiment 1. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (C) Experiment 1: 5AFC binocular rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (ML) a mostly left-oriented grating with some right-oriented lines, (M) a balanced left- and right-oriented grating (indicated by the absence of a key press), (MR) a mostly right-oriented grating with some left-oriented lines, or (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating. (D) Experiment 2: 4AFC superimposition versus piecemeal rivalry task. Participants were instructed to continuously indicate whether they were seeing (L) an exclusively left-oriented grating, (R) an exclusively right-oriented grating, pressing both (L + R) simultaneously to indicate they were seeing a piecemeal percept, or (M) a superimposition percept.
Figure 2
 
Partitioning original rivalry data into different dependent variables. (A) Observer's rivalry percept. (B) Ideal observer's key press response corresponding to percept. (C) Obtaining phase durations of overall mixed visibility We concatenated adjacent mixed percepts reported using the three mixed states in the original task to compute a new aggregated mixed percept state from which we extracted the median duration of mixed visibility.
Figure 2
 
Partitioning original rivalry data into different dependent variables. (A) Observer's rivalry percept. (B) Ideal observer's key press response corresponding to percept. (C) Obtaining phase durations of overall mixed visibility We concatenated adjacent mixed percepts reported using the three mixed states in the original task to compute a new aggregated mixed percept state from which we extracted the median duration of mixed visibility.
Figure 3
 
Patching-induced changes in overall fractions and median phase durations. From the top down, the five percept states are (a) exclusive percepts from the deprived eye, (b) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the deprived eye, (c) the balanced mixed percepts, (d) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the nondeprived eye, and (e) the exclusive percept from the nondeprived eye. (A) Overall fractions. The left column shows individual participants' baseline fraction durations for each percept plotted against their postdeprivation fraction durations; the right column illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation (with replacement) on the pooled normalized postbaseline values for each percept category. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline values. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (B) Median phase durations. See panel A for corresponding information. N = 13; *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < .001.
Figure 3
 
Patching-induced changes in overall fractions and median phase durations. From the top down, the five percept states are (a) exclusive percepts from the deprived eye, (b) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the deprived eye, (c) the balanced mixed percepts, (d) the mixed percepts biased in favor of the nondeprived eye, and (e) the exclusive percept from the nondeprived eye. (A) Overall fractions. The left column shows individual participants' baseline fraction durations for each percept plotted against their postdeprivation fraction durations; the right column illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation (with replacement) on the pooled normalized postbaseline values for each percept category. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline values. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. (B) Median phase durations. See panel A for corresponding information. N = 13; *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < .001.
Figure 4
 
Patching-induced changes in mixed visibility and perceptual eye dominance (A) Normalized postbaseline overall fraction of mixed visibility. Scatterplot (left) illustrating individual subjects' baseline fraction of mixed visibility (N = 13), plotted against their initial postdeprivation fraction of mixed visibility; middle panel illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation on the postbaseline differences. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline differences; right panel demonstrates individual normalized postbaseline overall fractions of mixed visibility at 0, 30, and 60 min after deprivation. The gray markers indicate the group mean. n = 5 (three women, age 24 ± 2.1). (B) Normalized postbaseline overall median duration mixed visibility. See panel A for corresponding information. (C) Normalized postbaseline perceptual eye dominance over the course of 1 hr after deprivation. Positive values indicate shifted bias in favor of the deprived eye. The perceptual eye ODI used to calculate these means utilized the median duration of the exclusive percepts from the deprived and nondeprived eyes. See panel A for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate means significantly shifted with respect to baseline. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < 0.001.
Figure 4
 
Patching-induced changes in mixed visibility and perceptual eye dominance (A) Normalized postbaseline overall fraction of mixed visibility. Scatterplot (left) illustrating individual subjects' baseline fraction of mixed visibility (N = 13), plotted against their initial postdeprivation fraction of mixed visibility; middle panel illustrates the output of a 1,000-iteration nonparametric bootstrapping implementation on the postbaseline differences. A Gaussian function was fit to a 20-bin histogram of the bootstrap distributions, illustrating the spread of the distributions. We used these bootstrap distributions to obtain 95% confidence intervals and the standard error (equivalent to the standard deviation of the bootstrap distribution) for the mean postbaseline differences; right panel demonstrates individual normalized postbaseline overall fractions of mixed visibility at 0, 30, and 60 min after deprivation. The gray markers indicate the group mean. n = 5 (three women, age 24 ± 2.1). (B) Normalized postbaseline overall median duration mixed visibility. See panel A for corresponding information. (C) Normalized postbaseline perceptual eye dominance over the course of 1 hr after deprivation. Positive values indicate shifted bias in favor of the deprived eye. The perceptual eye ODI used to calculate these means utilized the median duration of the exclusive percepts from the deprived and nondeprived eyes. See panel A for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate means significantly shifted with respect to baseline. Individual colored dots indicate unique participants. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05, **FDR-corrected p < 0.01, ***FDR-corrected p < 0.001.
Figure 5
 
PCA on median rivalry phase duration data. (A) Output of the PCA. The PCA was administered on baseline rivalry phase durations drawn from the reduced processed time series illustrated in Figure 2C. The components are statistically uncorrelated, pointing to three unique processes underlying the phase-duration data. The PCA coefficients indicate the degree to which each principal component (PCs 1–3) is associated with the median durations of each percept type. PC 1 is most closely associated with the median duration of mixed visibility, PC 2 is most closely associated with the median duration of complete perceptual suppression, and PC 3 is plausibly interpreted as ocular imbalance or perceptual eye dominance (see Methods for more information on the PCA). (B) Correlating baseline PCA scores with baseline binocular rivalry features. The x-axis corresponds to the z-normalized PC scores for each PC across subjects; the y-axis values indicate z-normalized values corresponding to the following baseline median phase duration data: PC 1, median duration of mixed visibility; PC 2, median duration of exclusive visibility (the arithmetic mean of the exclusive percepts' median durations); and PC 3, the ratio of the median durations of the exclusive percepts defined in Equation 1. PC scores are highly correlated with their respective binocular rivalry features. (C) Comparing postbaseline PC scores. Pre- and postpatching PC scores were obtained using the method outlined in Methods. PC scores indicate the degree to which each PC weighs on an individual's rivalry data. Each bar indicates the group M ± SEM. See panel C for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate significant interactions. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05.
Figure 5
 
PCA on median rivalry phase duration data. (A) Output of the PCA. The PCA was administered on baseline rivalry phase durations drawn from the reduced processed time series illustrated in Figure 2C. The components are statistically uncorrelated, pointing to three unique processes underlying the phase-duration data. The PCA coefficients indicate the degree to which each principal component (PCs 1–3) is associated with the median durations of each percept type. PC 1 is most closely associated with the median duration of mixed visibility, PC 2 is most closely associated with the median duration of complete perceptual suppression, and PC 3 is plausibly interpreted as ocular imbalance or perceptual eye dominance (see Methods for more information on the PCA). (B) Correlating baseline PCA scores with baseline binocular rivalry features. The x-axis corresponds to the z-normalized PC scores for each PC across subjects; the y-axis values indicate z-normalized values corresponding to the following baseline median phase duration data: PC 1, median duration of mixed visibility; PC 2, median duration of exclusive visibility (the arithmetic mean of the exclusive percepts' median durations); and PC 3, the ratio of the median durations of the exclusive percepts defined in Equation 1. PC scores are highly correlated with their respective binocular rivalry features. (C) Comparing postbaseline PC scores. Pre- and postpatching PC scores were obtained using the method outlined in Methods. PC scores indicate the degree to which each PC weighs on an individual's rivalry data. Each bar indicates the group M ± SEM. See panel C for corresponding information. Asterisks indicate significant interactions. *FDR-corrected p < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
Experiment 2: Patching-induced changes in superimposition versus piecemeal mixed visibility during rivalry. (A) Decay of patching-induced effect on fraction mixed visibility and MPR. The overall fraction of mixed visibility (red + green) is the sum of the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts (shown in green) and that of the piecemeal percepts (shown in red). The top rows of asterisked interactions indicate a significant increase in the fraction of mixed visibility at t0 and t15 with respect to baseline. The bottom row of asterisked interactions indicates an increase in the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts at t0, t15, and t60 with respect to baseline. Piecemeal percepts did not shift significantly with respect to baseline. (B) Decay of patching-induced effect on perceptual eye dominance. Perceptual eye dominance gradually recovers to baseline. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to baseline. *p < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
Experiment 2: Patching-induced changes in superimposition versus piecemeal mixed visibility during rivalry. (A) Decay of patching-induced effect on fraction mixed visibility and MPR. The overall fraction of mixed visibility (red + green) is the sum of the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts (shown in green) and that of the piecemeal percepts (shown in red). The top rows of asterisked interactions indicate a significant increase in the fraction of mixed visibility at t0 and t15 with respect to baseline. The bottom row of asterisked interactions indicates an increase in the absolute predominance of superimposition percepts at t0, t15, and t60 with respect to baseline. Piecemeal percepts did not shift significantly with respect to baseline. (B) Decay of patching-induced effect on perceptual eye dominance. Perceptual eye dominance gradually recovers to baseline. Asterisks indicate significant differences with respect to baseline. *p < 0.05.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×