We preregistered two hypotheses of interest prior to data collection, based on previously collected pilot data (n = 4), which are not otherwise used in this study. As a main effect, we expected modulation of ERPs that differentiated between the Layout Deviance of a stimulus (Surface Layout Repeat or Deviant from preceding presentations). In addition, we wished to see if any mismatch difference between Repeat and Deviant stimuli would interact with the degree of Prior Evidence, coded as the number of layout repetitions on immediately preceding trials.
Analysis of preliminary data highlighted two time intervals and regions of interest associated with a change in layout. These spatiotemporal windows of interest were preregistered as focal points of further analysis (see figures). The early window was from 200 to 250 ms following stimulus onset, at posterior electrodes in a band from PO3 through POz to PO4. Specifically, this region encompassed electrodes 66, 67, 71, 72, 76, 77, and 84 in the 128-electrode EGI HydroCel GSN system. This window of interest roughly corresponded with the spatial and temporal peak of the P2 component, as well as part of the N2 component. A second positive modulation was identified over a diffuse central and parietal area, with long-lasting effects peaking after 300 ms. Accordingly we defined this later window of interest as 300–400 ms after stimulus onset, across electrodes 7, 31, 54, 55, 79, 80, and 106, approximately the area surrounding Cz and CPz. While this region and time window appeared associated with the P300, and layout deviants showed more positivity relative to layout repeats, unsubtracted ERPs in this window showed negative voltages relative to the rest of the scalp. It should be noted that these windows of interest were determined a priori, and any spatial and temporal empirical effects could be expected to vary from these windows.
Average ERP voltages were calculated across electrodes and time points for each window of interest (as already stated). These mean voltages were then submitted to a four-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with factors Layout Deviance, Prior Evidence, Surface Layout, and Lit Surface. We tested for a main effect of Layout Deviance and any two-way interaction with that factor, plus a three-way interaction between Layout Deviance, Surface Layout, and Lit Surface. Further interactions between Prior Evidence and stimulus factors Surface Layout and Lit Surface were not considered.
As specific hypotheses, we expected a larger negativity in prediction-violating stimuli compared to prediction-confirming stimuli in the early window. We also expected the later window to show a positive difference in prediction-violating stimuli compared to prediction-confirming stimuli. Thus, we hypothesized a main effect of Layout Deviance for each window of interest. Further planned comparisons were performed for the effect of Prior Evidence on Layout Deviance, using a Tukey's HSD test for the difference in voltage means between Deviant and Repeat stimuli as predicted by Prior Evidence.
As the factor of Prior Evidence was unbalanced in number of trials in the ratio 4:2:1, the number of epochs available to compute voltage measures varied, potentially affecting their signal-to-noise ratios. A common strategy in this scenario is to randomly subsample from conditions with more trials (Cohen,
2014). Here, we instead used all available epochs for each condition, even if epoch counts were imbalanced. For confirmatory hypotheses, parallel analyses were performed using systematic sampling for epochs. For each level of the condition of Prior Evidence, epochs were sampled with a sampling interval of 1, 2, or 4, as appropriate, to select approximately 100 epochs per level. The results of these analyses did not noticeably differ from the nonsubsampled results described as follows.