Sudden movements in the periphery of our visual field often elicit immediate goal-directed eye movements (Gellman & Carl,
1991; Dorr, Martinetz, Gegenfurtner, & Barth,
2010). Due to the specific organization of the primate visual system, it is necessary to align the visual axis of our eyes with the moving object to gain highest foveal acuity information (Kowler,
2011; Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner,
2011; Kowler, Aitkin, Ross, Santos, & Zhao,
2014; Gegenfurtner,
2016). Typically, rapid saccadic eye movements with peak velocities up to 500–700 deg/s are first initiated to direct the eyes toward the moving object, so that its retinal projection lands close to the foveal region. Visual tracking responses are then continued with slow eye rotations, called smooth pursuit eye movements. Under optimal conditions pursuit eye movements stabilize the image of a predictably moving target on the fovea after about 200–300 ms. Pursuit minimizes the residual image movement on the retina, the retinal slip and benefits the perception of moving targets (Ludvigh & Miller,
1958; Methling & Wernicke,
1968; Brown,
1972a,
1972b,
1972c; Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner,
2009; Lisberger,
2015). The precision and accuracy of visual tracking responses depends on many factors such as the predictability of the target movement (Barnes & Asselman,
1991; Orban de Xivry & Lefèvre,
2007; Kowler et al.,
2014), target contrast (Doma & Hallett,
1988; Ludwig, Gilchrist, & McSorley,
2004; Spering, Kerzel, Braun, Hawken, & Gegenfurtner,
2005; Braun et al.,
2008), speed (Carl & Gellman,
1987; Kowler & McKee,
1987; Osborne, Lisberger, & Bialek,
2005; Rasche & Gegenfurtner,
2009), starting position with respect to the fovea (Lisberger & Westbrook,
1985), and its defining motion properties (Hawken & Gegenfurtner,
2001; Lisi & Cavanagh,
2015).