Recently Chetverikov, Campana, and Kristjánsson (
2016,
2017a,
2017b,
2018) introduced a new approach for studying internal representations of feature distributions, using priming in visual search (Kristjánsson & Campana,
2010). The well-known “priming of pop out” effect involves a decrease in response time after repeated presentation of target and distractor features (Maljkovic & Nakayama,
1994; see Kristjánsson & Ásgeirsson,
2019 for a recent review). Switching the target and distractor features leads to an increase in response time that is even larger than for new target and distractor features (Kristjánsson & Driver,
2008). If a target is blue and distractors are red, search is slowed down more if the target becomes red than if it would switch to green. Chetverikov et al. (
2016) used this role-reversal effect to assess the internal representation of orientation distributions by probing the target at different points in feature space, revealing the internal model of the distractor distributions. Their observers saw search displays containing 36 lines drawn from a predefined distribution and observers searched for an oddly oriented line. After a certain number of learning trials with a constant distractor distribution, the target was placed at different probe points within and around the previous distractor distribution. They found that search time was slower when the target feature was suddenly drawn from within the preceding distractor distribution, compared to when it was drawn from the feature space outside the preceding distractor distribution. Chetverikov et al. were therefore able to use the target as a probe of the learned distractor distribution. Moreover, they found that response times, as a function of the distance between the learned distractor mean and the target, resembled the shape of the distractor distribution. RT functions that followed Gaussian distractor distributions monotonically decreased, and RT functions following uniform distributions consisted of a flat part followed by a linear decrease. Furthermore, their results revealed that the visual system also encodes skewed feature distributions, resulting in skewed RT functions. Observers needed only a few exposures to the distractor distributions to develop an internal feature representation of them, but the minimum number of repeated search displays needed to encode the distribution depended on its complexity. While two to three repetitions were sufficient for a Gaussian or a uniform distribution, observers needed additional learning trials to encode a bimodal distribution (Chetverikov et al.,
2017a). Subsequent experiments showed that a minimum number of exemplars (set size in visual search displays) are needed for robust distribution encoding (Chetverikov, Campana, & Kristjánsson,
2017c). Encoding the shape of feature distributions has been shown to occur for both color (Chetverikov et al.,
2017b) and orientation (Chetverikov et al.,
2016) separately. That is, while the explicit judgments of summary statistics do not seem to reveal any pick-up of more complex distribution properties (e.g., Atchley & Anderson,
1995; Dakin & Watt,
1997), implicit distribution learning during visual search shows that observers implicitly encode the distribution shape, indicating that the method of probing distribution representations may affect the results.