Subsequent studies considered the relationship between perceived two-dimensional (2D) density and numerosity. There is some controversy about whether the mechanisms for perceiving the two are the same or not. Burr and Ross (
2008) found that numerosity is a primal visual property because it is sensitive to adaptation and that observers estimate numerosity independently of density. This finding was challenged by Durgin (
2008) who had shown that adaptation is determined by texture density and not numerosity (Durgin,
1995). Other studies claimed that numerosity and density are not independent. In a task comparing two separate 2D patches, Dakin, Tibber, Greenwood, Kingdom, and Morgan (
2011) and Tibber, Greenwood, and Dakin (
2012) found similar just noticeable differences (JNDs) in density and numerosity discrimination. They also showed perceived density and number were biased by increases in area, such that larger areas were perceived as denser and more numerous. Bell, Manson, Edwards, and Meso (
2015) also found that perceived density was biased by increases in area, but did not find that perceived numerosity was biased by increases in area. They also showed that neither perceived density nor numerosity were biased by increases in volume. Several groups measured Weber fractions for numerosity discrimination. Burr and Ross (
2008) and Ross and Burr (
2010) showed that numerosity obeys Weber's law for low densities. For high densities, Anobile, Cicchini, and Burr (
2014), Anobile, Turi, Cicchini, and Burr (
2015) and Cicchini, Anobile, and Burr (
2016) found that the thresholds increased only with the square root of density, corresponding to a decreasing Weber fraction. From this, they inferred that there exist different mechanisms for perceiving numerosity and density at low and high densities.