Another important way of relating stereopsis, BR, and DM is through the concept of sensory eye dominance (SED). One way to determine SED is to quantify the amount of interocular suppression from each eye and calculate the difference in strength between the eyes. For example, Bagolini striated lenses viewed with a sliding scale of neutral density filters over one eye can be used to assess the suppression from each eye toward the other. Thus, it can provide a measurement of SED and ultimately serve as an independent comparison to BR and DM. Furthermore, having a simple but graded measurement of SED is advantageous because the magnitude of SED varies along a continuum (Ooi & He,
2001). Importantly, high SED symmetry has been reported to correlate with good stereoacuity, binocular disparity thresholds, and time-dependent depth detection tasks in random-dot stereograms (Xu, He, & Ooi,
2010). Thus, individuals with a small SED generally demonstrate symmetrical input from both eyes with normal stereoacuity, and individuals with a larger SED may demonstrate more asymmetrical input from both eyes, resulting in weak or possibly no stereoscopic capabilities (Ooi & He,
2001; Sireteanu & Fronius,
1981; Xu, He, & Ooi,
2011). Moreover, McKee and Harrad (
1993) found, in normal subjects, that the fusional suppression of the monocular information was symmetrical and that neither eye had a strong access to the monocular information. However, in SA subjects, this relationship tends to break down as the asymmetry between eyes increases with monocular access enhanced for the dominant eye and the weaker eye becoming suppressed. Therefore, having a larger SED may result in the loss of binocular integration and stereopsis (Sireteanu & Fronius,
1981). Similar effects exist for BR, with which a strong SED imbalance is correlated with the relative predominance of the dominant eye (Xu et al.,
2010). Thus, rivalry may be driven by the same overall imbalance in interocular inhibition that causes a significant disruption of binocular vision (Dieter, Sy, & Blake,
2016; Xu et al.,
2010). Furthermore, normal observers with symmetrical binocularity, viewing equivalent contrasts in each eye, report the perception of the stimuli as being equal (Ding, Klein, & Levi,
2013). In contrast, amblyopic subjects, for example, require more equivalent contrast presented to their nondominant eye to perceive an equivalent contrast (Ding et al.,
2013). Because this occurs for low spatial frequencies typically detected normally by amblyopic eyes, it suggests an asymmetry in their binocularity rather than a loss of monocular sensitivity.