To our knowledge, the binocular latency and monocular latency from SSVEP studies have previously rarely been discussed. In contrast, the P100 peak time of binocular and monocular VEPs have been intensively discussed, and the results typically show shorter binocular latencies than monocular latencies (Johansson & Jakobsson,
1993; McCulloch & Skarf,
1991; McKerral et al.,
1995; Wanger & Nilsson,
1978). In contrast with these studies, the current observations showed no significant difference in binocular and monocular latency. One plausible reason for this discrepancy was the different measurement of latency. That is, the VEPs only measured the latency onset at the peaks, while the current study measured the latency by use of an entire waveform. In addition, the stimulus conditions might account for this discrepancy. For example, Bagolini, Porciatti, and Falsini's study from
1988 used a range of spatial frequencies of checkerboard stimuli and found that the shorter latency for binocular viewing compared to monocular viewing only occurred at 0.6–2 cycles per degree but no phase was shorter at spatial frequencies higher than 2 c/d (Bagolini et al.,
1988). In addition to latency information, we found that the binocular envelope amplitude was larger than the envelope amplitude from stimulation of the dominant eye or the nondominant eye (see
Figure 6C). This finding was generally in line with most previous studies, in which the performance of both eyes is enhanced compared to one eye in both psychophysical and electrophysiological studies (Apkarian, Nakayama, & Tyler,
1981; Baker, Lygo, Meese, & Georgeson,
2018; Baker, Wallis, Georgeson, & Meese,
2012; Campbell & Green,
1965; Di Summa et al.,
1997; Ding & Levi,
2017; Ding & Sperling,
2006; Meese, Georgeson, & Baker,
2006; Plainis, Petratou, Giannakopoulou, Atchison, & Tsilimbaris,
2011; Richard, Chadnova, & Baker,
2018; Tobimatsu & Kato,
1996). The binocular summation ratio is widely used in most previous studies when measuring the binocular advantage over monocular sensitivities. In early psychophysical work, Campbell and Green (
1965) reported that the binocular contrast sensitivity was about 1.4 times enhancement that of monocular sensitivity, indicating a binocular summation ratio of 1.4 (Campbell & Green,
1965). However, this ratio has been reported to be significantly greater than the canonical value of 1.4, with an approximate range from 1.4 to 2 (Baker et al.,
2018; Baker et al.,
2012; Meese et al.,
2006; Richard et al.,
2018). From the pattern of our results, the amplitudes under binocular condition were approximately 2.2 ± 0.16 (
M ±
SEM) times (i.e., a ratio of binocular amplitude to the averaged monocular amplitudes, averaged across participants) larger than those obtained in the monocular condition. This result supports the range from recent theories instead of the canonical ratio (i.e., a binocular summation ratio of 1.4). This was also consistent with the range from 1.3 to 2.5 reported in most previous electrophysiological studies (Apkarian et al.,
1981; Di Summa et al.,
1997; Plainis et al.,
2011; Tobimatsu & Kato,
1996). The current finding implies an approximately linear summation process occurs during binocular viewing.