Age-related macular degeneration (AMD) is the most common cause of vision loss in the developed world and affects nearly 7% of individuals over age 40 in the United States alone (Klein et al.,
2011). Individuals with macular degeneration often experience a loss of their central visual field, with only peripheral vision remaining. Because of the prevalence of macular degeneration and its potentially devastating effects on the central visual field, it is important to understand how different aspects of visual function are affected in this population. One aspect important in daily life is the perception of motion (Nakayama,
1985). Some studies suggest that individuals with central field loss (CFL) are adept at using peripheral motion information, as in the case of vection (Tarita-Nistor, González, Markowitz, Lillakas, & Steinbach,
2008) and heading perception (Odom, Mali, & Leys,
2010). This view is consistent with a body of work that demonstrates the periphery's sensitivity to motion processing, especially at faster speeds (McKee & Nakayama,
1984).
In contrast to these findings, a study (Eisenbarth, MacKeben, Poggel, & Strasburger,
2008) aimed specifically at testing motion sensitivity in CFL found elevated contrast thresholds to judge motion direction. Specifically, they reported that motion contrast sensitivity was significantly reduced for eccentricities up to 20° in individuals with CFL, compared to age-matched and young controls. Thus, it appears that motion sensitivity is impaired well beyond the macula, which extends to less than 10° eccentricity (Strasburger, Rentschler, & Jüttner,
2011). However, the actual eccentricities of the stimulus in Eisenbarth et al. (
2008) are uncertain, as the authors did not measure the eccentricity of the peripheral retinal locus (PRL), or monitor eye movements during the task. Although it is possible that their findings reveal deficits beyond the macula for stimuli close to contrast threshold, other work has found no differences in absolute detection thresholds at eccentricities greater than 10° in individuals with AMD (Sunness, Massof, Johnson, Finkelstein, & Fine,
1985). Thus, the combination of low stimulus contrasts and unknown stimulus eccentricity does not quite address whether motion perception for high contrast stimuli is impaired beyond the region of the scotoma.
Our own studies show that smooth pursuit in the CFL population is impaired compared to age-matched controls (Shanidze, Fusco, Potapchuk, Heinen, & Verghese,
2016); (Shanidze, Heinen, & Verghese,
2017). In these studies, we examined smooth pursuit during monocular and binocular viewing, and found that performance was impaired under all conditions, regardless of scotoma size or binocular overlap. Since smooth pursuit is a velocity-driven behavior (Beutter and Stone,
1998; Churchland & Lisberger,
2001), one might hypothesize that deficiencies in the perception of speed or direction might lead to this impairment. It is also possible that the lower gains may simply be due to the periphery's insensitivity to the lower speeds (McKee & Nakayama,
1984) characteristic of retinal slip. Therefore, the question remains whether this deficiency is due to other CFL-related limitations, such as a limited oculomotor range (Stahl,
2001; Whittaker, Budd, & Cummings,
1988), or an impairment of peripheral motion processing.
To probe potential impairments of motion processing in CFL, we set out to systematically measure sensitivity to speed and direction of motion. With a wealth of evidence suggesting that normal aging causes an impairment of motion perception and eye movements (Eisenbarth et al.,
2008; Odom et al.,
2010; Sharpe & Sylvester,
1978), we compared speed and direction discrimination in individuals with CFL to age-matched and young controls. We asked individuals to make speed and direction discrimination judgments while fixating a central target. To draw a direct parallel to our smooth pursuit experiments (Shanidze et al.,
2017), we chose to stay in a similar velocity range of 5°/s and 10°/s. Although most studies of visual function in macular degeneration have measured visual function monocularly and related it to disease progression in that eye, we wanted to understand if motion perception is impaired under real-world circumstances. Therefore, we used a full-field, high-contrast stimulus that participants viewed binocularly, better approximating real-world conditions. We mapped out the binocular scotoma of our participants to understand the relationship of scotoma extent to motion perception under binocular viewing conditions. We hypothesized that under full-field conditions, individuals with CFL would perform similarly to age-matched controls, as in other studies comparing the two groups (e.g., Odom et al.,
2010). Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that individuals with CFL and age-matched controls had similar performance in speed and direction discrimination, but were significantly worse than the young control group.
Having measured the eccentricity of the preferred retinal locus, we ensured viewing eccentricity from fixation (foveal or eccentric) by monitoring the eye movements of our participants. As scotomas varied significantly in size and location across participants, we examined whether restricting the information to a narrow region of the periphery affected motion perception. We varied the eccentricity (up to 15°) of narrow bands of the motion stimulus, and had our young control participants perform the task. Our results show that performance across tested eccentricities remained constant for the young control group.
We also used a binocular perimetry technique to measure the extent of the binocular scotoma on the same screen on which motion stimuli were presented. As we used a two-spatial alternative paradigm, with the stimuli to be discriminated on the left and right of fixation, we wondered whether individuals with CFL, whose scotoma fell more on one side than the other, had impaired motion perception on that side. Examination of the pattern of correct responses to the test stimulus on each side suggests that performance is not related to binocular scotoma size or location. Furthermore, we did not find a relationship between speed and direction discrimination thresholds and visual acuity or PRL distance of the better eye, suggesting that motion perception in the periphery is not affected by loss of central visual field.