In 1967, Yarbus demonstrated that scene exploration could differ greatly according to the task at hand (Tatler, Wade, Kwan, Findlay, & Velichkovsky,
2010). He concluded that a visual task has an effect on someone's gaze. Indeed, cognitive processes can be observed through eye movements and offer a wealth of information related to internal processes (Itti,
2015; Coutrot, Hsiao, & Chan,
2018). Inference from gaze data consists in deducing subjective characteristics solely from ocular data, such as age (Le Meur et al.,
2017b), gender (Coutrot, Binetti, Harrison, Mareschal, & Johnston,
2016; Sammaknejad, Pouretemad, Eslahchi, Salahirad, & Alinejad,
2017), mental states and traits (Liao, Zhang, Zhu, & Ji,
2005; Hoppe, Loetscher, Morey, & Bulling,
2015; Yamada & Kobayashi,
2017; Hoppe, Loetscher, Morey, & Bulling,
2018), expertise and skill proficiency (Eivazi & Bednarik,
2011; Boccignone, Ferraro, Crespi, Robino, & de'Sperati,
2014; Tien et al.,
2014; Kolodziej, Majkowski, Francuz, Rak, & Augustynowicz,
2018), and neurological disorders (Kupas, Harangi, Czifra, & Andrassy,
2017; Terao, Fukuda, & Hikosaka,
2017).It has proven useful in identifying autism spectrum disorder (Pierce et al.,
2016), fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (Tseng, Paolozza, Munoz, Reynolds, & Itti,
2013), dementia (Zhang et al.,
2016; Beltrán, García-Vázquez, Benois-Pineau, Gutierrez-Robledo, & Dartigues,
2018), dyslexia (Benfatto et al.,
2016), anxiety (Abbott, Shirali, Haws, & Lack,
2017), mental fatigue (Yamada & Kobayashi,
2017), and other disorders. It has also been applied to task detection (Borji & Itti,
2014; Haji-Abolhassani & Clark,
2014; Kanan, Ray, Bseiso, Hsiao, & Cottrell,
2014; Boisvert & Bruce,
2016). In addition gaze is also utilized as a biometric clue (Holland & Komogortsev,
2011; Cantoni, Galdi, Nappi, Porta, & Riccio,
2015).