Across both groups, participants appropriately used the endogenous pre-cue to attend to the target, as indicated by behavioral performance: the valid cue led to better performance than the invalid cue in both accuracy (
d′ (percent correct in parentheses) for valid:
M = 0.58 (60.9%); invalid
M = 0.084 (52%)) and RT (valid
M = 443.5 ms; invalid
M = 464 ms). A 2 × 2 (pre-cue group x cue validity) mixed-design ANOVA revealed that these differences were significant (accuracy:
F(1, 26) = 31.13,
p < 0.001,
\(\eta _G^2\) = 0.33; see
Figure 2A; RT:
F(1,26) = 7.69,
p = 0.01,
\(\eta _G^2\) = 0.03; see
Figure 2B).
Critically, both groups performed equally well on the task, F(1, 26) = 0.81, p = 0.38, \(\eta _G^2\) = 0.02, and we did not observe a significant pre-cue group by cue validity interaction within accuracy, F(1, 26) = 0.39, p = 0.54, \(\eta _G^2\) = 0.006. Furthermore, a pair of within-participants t tests revealed that both groups exhibited significant attention effects in accuracy: FC group, t(13) = 4.29, p = 0.00088, d = 1.15; SC group, t(13) = 3.80, p = 0.0022, d = 1.02. Similarly, RTs were statistically matched between groups, F(1, 26) = 0.64, p = 0.43, \(\eta _G^2\) = 0.02, and there was no significant interaction between factors, F(1,26) = 0.92, p = 0.35, \(\eta _G^2\) = 0.004. Within-participants t tests further revealed that attention effects in RT were significant for the FC group, t(13) = 2.62, p = 0.02, d = 0.70, and marginal in the SC group, t(13) = 1.99, p = 0.068, d = 0.53. Thus, both the spatial and feature pre-cues elicited attention effects of similar magnitudes.