Despite the more difficult ratios, participants performed the task with well above chance accuracy (accuracy
M: 87.3%,
SD: 5.6%,
n = 27).
Figure 3 shows the proportion of trials that participants selected the right stimulus at different numerical and orientation coherence ratios. Again, the numerical ratio effect is illustrated by the curved functions in
Figure 3 and the orientation coherence effects are apparent by the separation of the functions by coherence. We fit the regression model (
Equation 1) to the choice data of each participant. As in
Experiment 1, we found an effect of numerical ratio, β
Num M: 6.6,
SEM: 0.37, t(26) = 17.9,
p < 0.001, and an effect of orientation coherence/ variance,
\({\beta _{{\sigma ^2}}}\) M: –0.3,
SEM: 0.03, t(26) = –10.8,
p < 0.001. We confirmed this finding using a mixed-effects model with participant treated as a random effect; we found a significant fixed-effect of numerical ratio, β
Num = 3.7, t(16197) = 19.6,
p < 0.001, and variance ratio
\({\beta _{{\sigma ^2}}}\) = –0.15, t(16197) = –10.8,
p < 0.001. Twenty-six of 27 participants had a negative
\({\beta _{{\sigma ^2}}}\) coefficient. On average, the most coherent arrays seemed to be 7.6% more numerous than the least coherent arrays, sufficient to make 15 coherent objects appear equal to 16 variable objects. When the numerosities presented were equal but one array was maximally coherent and the other was maximally variable, participants indicated that the more coherent array was greater 68% of the time. This is equivalent to the comparison of the most and least coherent arrays in
Figure 1.
We tested whether precision on the numerical comparison task was correlated with the effect of orientation coherence at the participant level. We found a significant negative correlation between βNum and \({\beta _{{\sigma ^2}}}\), meaning that greater precision on numerical comparison was associated with a stronger effect of coherence (r = –0.47, p = 0.014).