In addition to our main research question, we tested to what extent individual differences in the strength of perceptual filling-in are related to individual differences in the strength of voluntary mental imagery. Both filling-in and visual imagery are instances of nonretinal vision—perceiving something that was not a direct consequence of external stimulation. To the best of our knowledge, previous studies have not looked at the relationship between mental imagery and perceptual filling-in. However, several studies have investigated the relationship between mental imagery and nonretinal vision other than filling-in. For example,
Grzeczkowski, Clarke, Francis, Mast, and Herzog (2017) found that mental imagery strength only correlated with the strength of the Ponzo illusion out of several visual illusions the authors tested, suggesting illusory percepts are not necessarily related to the same mechanisms as voluntary mental imagery. More vivid mental imagery (as self-reported using the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire [VVIQ],
Marks, 1995;
Marks, 1973) has been shown to lead to stronger influence of imagery on subsequent perception in a binocular rivalry paradigm—the imagined pattern was more likely to be dominant (
Pearson, Clifford, & Tong, 2008, but see
Dijkstra, Hinne, Bosch, & van Gerven, 2019, who found an effect of vividness of the imagined stimulus on a particular trial, but not the VVIQ scores). Other studies have looked at whether mental imagery is related to hallucinations. For example,
Shine, Keogh, O'Callaghan, Muller, Lewis, and Pearson (2015) found that stronger mental imagery is associated with more visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. In a healthy population,
Salge, Pollmann, and Reeder (2019) found a correlation between imagery and pareidolia, suggesting that stronger mental imagery can lead to a greater chance of misperceiving an externally presented stimulus. Furthermore, the size of the primary visual cortex has been linked to differences in both imagery (
Bergmann, Genç, Kohler, Singer, & Pearson, 2015) and strength of visual illusions (
Schwarzkopf & Rees, 2013;
Schwarzkopf, Song, & Rees, 2011), with smaller V1 being associated with stronger imagery and stronger Ebbinghaus and Ponzo illusion perception. This suggests, albeit indirectly, that strength of mental imagery might be associated with the strength of, at least some, illusory percepts. It is not clear how perceptual filling-in may relate to hallucinatory experiences or misperceptions in visual illusions, and therefore we did not have a specific hypothesis on whether perceptual filling-in relates to visual imagery, but to explore this question, participants completed the VVIQ as part of the study.