After pre-processing, we computed a Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) for individual channels to transform waveforms into the frequency domain. With a sampling rate at 1000 Hz and zero-padding, we computed amplitude spectrums (µV) at values ranging from 0 to 500 Hz in increments of 0.1 Hz. The occipital channel (Oz) had the highest power for all stimulated frequencies (6.9 Hz, 55.5 Hz, and 62.5 Hz) in all conditions; thus, we focused on the Oz channel in the following analysis. The base-10 log-transformed amplitude spectrum was averaged across trials, and then grand-averaged across all participants (
Figure 10).
To test whether the flickers induced the corresponding SSVEPs, we conducted separate rmANOVAs on three frequency amplitudes of interest (7 Hz, 55.5 Hz, and 62.5 Hz) with flicker condition as a factor. Note that in the 7 Hz power comparisons in the following analyses, we compared the amplitude at 6.9 Hz in the visible-flicker condition with the amplitude at 7 Hz in the other two conditions because the stimulated frequency in the visible-flicker condition was 6.9 Hz under the 0.1 Hz resolution. For simplicity, we refer to this comparison as between “amplitudes at 7 Hz” in the following text.
At 7 Hz, Mauchly's test of sphericity on the amplitudes from three conditions was significant (p = 0.03); thus, we used the Greenhouse-Geisser correction on the following tests. There was a significant effect of flicker condition on amplitudes at 7 Hz (F(1.46, 23.32) = 132.78, p < 0.001, Ƞ2p = 0.89). Three two-tailed planned comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed that the amplitude of 6.9 Hz in the flicker-visible condition (M = 0.03, SD = 0.32) was significantly larger than that in the flicker-invisible condition (M = -0.88, SD = 0.29, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 2.86) and control conditions (M = -0.82, SD = 0.24, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 3.31). However, the amplitude of 7 Hz in the flicker-invisible condition, which represented its beat amplitude, was not significantly larger than that in the control condition (p = 0.44). At 55.5 Hz, the test of sphericity was not violated. There was a significant effect of flicker condition on amplitude at 55.5 Hz (F(2, 32) = 15.16, p < 0.001, Ƞ2p = 0.49). Three two-tailed planned comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed that power in the flicker-invisible condition (M = -1.60, SD = 0.39) was significantly larger than the control condition (M = -2.11, SD = 0.44, p = 0.002, Cohen's d = 1.00). The flicker-visible condition (M = -1.54, SD = 0.37) was also significantly larger than the control (p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.27). There was no significant difference between flicker visible and invisible conditions. At 62.5 Hz, there was no significant difference among the three conditions (F(2, 32) = 1.43, p = 0.25); thus, we conducted no further comparisons.
We further examined the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) because it could enhance SSVEP peaks for visualization (
Vialatte, Maurice, Dauwels, & Cichocki, 2010). We calculated the SNR by taking the value at each frequency bin divided by the average value of the 20 neighboring bins (
Boremanse, Norcia, & Rossion, 2013). As shown in
Figure 11, results were similar to the previous analysis. In post hoc two-tailed comparisons between conditions with Bonferroni corrections, the SNR of the flicker-invisible condition (
M = 2.57, SD = 1.87) was significantly larger than that of the control condition (
M = 0.95, SD = 0.72,
p = 0.018, Cohen's d = 0.77). The flicker-visible condition that showed significantly larger power also had larger SNR (
M = 2.81, SD = 1.78) than that of the control (
p = 0.002, Cohen's d = 1.03).
Finally, we computed the ITC to examine the direct measurement of phase alignment when neural entrainment occurs.
Zoefel et al. (2018) differentiated neural entrainment from repetitive ERPs, suggesting that neural entrainment does not necessarily induce larger power at the stimulated frequency as repetitive ERPs do; rather, it entails phase alignment of endogenous neural oscillation and stimulation. Therefore, ITC analysis that measures the phase coherence provides a better account of neural oscillation strength apart from a power analysis. We, therefore, conducted ITC analysis by taking phase angles from FFT outputs, averaging them across trials following the formula below (
Cohen, 2014, p. 244).
\begin{equation*}ITP{C_{tf}} = \left| {{n^{ - 1}}\mathop \sum \limits_{r = 1}^n {e^{i{k_{tfr}}}}} \right|\end{equation*}
The resulting values were bound between 0 and 1, with 1 referring to the completely identical phase angles across trials, and 0 representing the completely uniform distributed phase angles. We then averaged ITC values across individuals and plotted the ITC spectra by frequency (
Figure 12).
An rmANOVA was conducted on three frequencies of interest to compare ITC values across conditions. At 7 Hz, there was a significant effect of flicker condition on ITC values (F(1.20, 19.23) = 113.91, p < 0.001, Ƞ2p = 0.88). Three two-tailed planned comparisons with Bonferroni corrections found that the 6.9 Hz ITC in the flicker-visible condition (M = 0.71, SD = 0.22) was significantly larger than the 7 Hz ITC in the flicker-invisible condition (M = 0.19, SD = 0.10, p < 0.001) and control condition (M = 0.21, SD = 0.09, p < 0.001). The 7 Hz ITC in the flicker-invisible condition was not significantly different from that of the control condition. At 55.5 Hz, the test of sphericity was not violated, and there was a significant effect of flicker condition on ITC (F(2, 32) = 7.07, p = 0.003, Ƞ2p = 0.31). Three two-tailed planned comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed that the flicker-invisible condition ITC (M = 0.18, SD = 0.15) was significantly larger than the control condition ITC (M = 0.07, SD = 0.05, p = 0.03, Cohen's d = 0.70). In addition, the flicker-visible condition ITC (M = 0.21, SD = 0.13) was significantly larger than the control condition ITC (p = 0.003, Cohen's d = 0.97). At 62.5 Hz, there was no main effect of condition on ITC (F(2, 32) = 2.43, p = 0.10); thus, no further comparisons were conducted. The ITC results were consistent with the power analysis.