Previous work found that the human visual system might use image contrast as an image variable to initiate percepts of transparency and to assign transmittance to transparent surfaces (
Singh & Anderson, 2002). A further study with more realistic images shows reducing contrast of the background decreases the perceived transparency of the overlaying filter (
Robilotto & Zaidi, 2004) and studies on volumetric translucency also reveal significant effects of image contrast (
Fleming & Bülthoff, 2005;
Motoyoshi, 2010). Specifically, root mean square (RMS) contrast in high spatial frequency components in diffuse object images are suggested to be important for perceptual translucency in (
Motoyoshi, 2010). A more recent work suggests global contrast is not related to translucency perception (
Nagai et al., 2013). Here, we examine the relationship between image contrast, geometric sharpness, and the perceived translucency using our stimuli. To compare with the perceptual results, we first compute the difference in Michelson contrast between the target and the candidate match images ((
Imax −
Imin)/(
Imax +
Imin)) and use this metric to make predictions of densities in a similar way as that has been described above.
Figure
13 plots the predicted density based on the difference in Michelson contrast between target and match images for all experimental conditions. First, image contrast could discriminate different optical densities as well as the perceptual data. Second, the effect of geometric smoothness resembles perceptual data for the shallow relief such that, as blur increases, the predicted density decreases (see left two columns in
Figure 13. However, as the relief height increases, the effect of blur on predicted density is diminished if not reversed (see right two columns in
Figure 13 in bottom rows), whereas in the perceptual data, increasing geometric smoothness would cause the matched density to decrease across all reliefs (see
Figure 9 and
Figure 11). Hence, for the high relief conditions, the predictions from image contrast is opposite the perception data, suggesting observers are not using only image contrasts to judge translucent appearance.
To further examine the interaction between image contrast and geometric smoothness, we also plotted both the Michelson and RMS contrasts of all target images in the supplementary materials (
Supplementary Figure 1S and
Supplementary Figure 2S). First, consistent with previous findings, both image contrast measurements correlate strongly with optical densities such that increasing density will result in increased contrast, suggesting that image contrast is an important cue for perceived translucency. However, the relationship between contrast and geometric sharpness is a bit more complicated. For images with low relief, increasing geometric smoothness (blur) tends to slightly lower contrast. In contrast, for images with higher relief and especially higher densities, geometric smoothness tends to increase image contrast.
The interaction between geometric sharpness (or smoothness) and image contrast can be seen from examples shown in
Figure 6 and
Figure 10. For lower-relief conditions (e.g., images in
Figure 6, left), increasing geometric smoothness does not change much of the apparent image contrast. In fact, smoothing geometry might result in slightly reduced contrast owing to the dispersing of pixels with extreme intensities around the edge. For higher relief (
Figure 6, bottom right), especially for higher densities, it is speculated that increasing geometric smoothness increases image contrast owing to the expansion of darker or brighter image regions around the edge. The smoothed edges of a high relief object will cast more shadows and becomes slightly more glossy and brighter than sharp edges (e.g., the leftmost letter “t” in
Figure 6 [right] becomes brighter than the images on the left). Hence, images with rounder edges tend to have higher contrast than images with sharper edges when the relief is sufficiently high (such as 2.0 mm and 2.5 mm conditions corresponding to contrast values shown in yellow and lines in fourth and fifth columns in
Supplementary Figures 1S and
2S). These effects of smoothness on contrast could be even stronger for the images with negative reliefs as well (
Figure 10). Owing to this interaction between image contrast and relief heights, the predictions generated from image contrast cannot fully explain the effects we found in perception. Overall, this analysis suggests that even though image contrast could be useful for the appearance of translucency owing to changes in optical densities, it cannot explain the effects of geometric sharpness on translucency that we found in our perceptual data. Perhaps, future work should be investigated whether multiscale contrast could be a cue for translucency.