Following suggestions that perceptual learning may take place even without attention, that is, when participants are performing a different task, we tested for implicit performance improvement in our experimental paradigm.
We trained eight MTurk participants on one task, when the stimulus for the second task was present, but irrelevant to performance of the assigned task. Each observer participated in 17 sessions over 6 weeks, each Monday, Wednesday, Friday, as follows. Four observers (two men, two women) did five sessions of array mean discrimination, judging which of two successively presented arrays had a more clockwise mean orientation, in which the base orientation was 60o or 70o, and the difference in mean orientation between the arrays was parameter ORDA, ±2o, ±4o, ±6o, ±8o, or ±12o. One of the two arrays also had a bar with an outlier orientation in one of 12 locations, with an orientation difference, ORDO, from the mean of that array of ±15o, ±20o, or ±30o. Despite the presence of the uninformative outlier, observers were instructed to respond only according to the mean orientation of the arrays. Following completion of five sessions, we asked these participants to switch to nine sessions of the outlier detection task. Again, stimulus arrays had different mean orientations and one had an outlier, with the same stimulus parameters as in the first five sessions. However, now the observers were asked to perform the alternate task, that is, to detect the outlier, and array mean orientation was irrelevant. Finally, the participants were instructed to switch back to perform mean orientation discrimination for another three sessions.
Four other participants (three male, one female) performed the complementary tasks. They began with five sessions of the outlier detection task, whereas the two arrays irrelevantly had different mean orientations. Then they switched to nine sessions of the mean orientation task, and presence of the outlier was irrelevant. Finally, they performed three more sessions of outlier detection.
It is important to note that the irrelevant task could not help participants’ performance because it was random, and thus irrelevant to the task. When performance was of the mean discrimination task, the outlier could appear in the more or the less clockwise oriented array equally, and when performance was of the outlier detection task, the more clockwise oriented array could be that with or without the outlier. Outlier orientation difference (ORDO) was measured from the mean of the array in which it was present and was either more clockwise or more counterclockwise than the mean.
The results are demonstrated separately for performance of the mean discrimination task (
Figure 6 top, bottom) and for performance of the outlier detection (
Figure 7 top, bottom).
Figures 6 and
7 top show performance accuracy, and
Figures 6 and
7 bottom show performance RT. To enable comparison of performance, in each graph we mix performance from the two groups of participants for the same task.
Figure 6 top shows performance of mean discrimination for the first five sessions and last three sessions of the group who performed mean discrimination first (and last), as well as the mean discrimination performance by the other group who performed this task in their sixth to 14th sessions. Performance after switching task (sessions 6–10: 0.70) is poorer than initially (sessions 1–5: 0.77 for the other group;
t-test:
p < 0.0001). There is significant interference for performing and learning mean discrimination after considerable practice with the same stimuli and performing the outlier detection task. During performance of the first five sessions of outlier detection, the differences in mean orientation between the two arrays was irrelevant, and perception of this difference seems to have been suppressed actively (although subconsciously) by the participants. This suppression led to reduced mean discrimination performance in sessions six to 14, unlike the case in Experiment 2,
Figure 5, in which there was no implicit presence of the secondary task elements. In addition, when switching back and forth, the group who began with mean discrimination had to do some more learning after having performed outlier detection in the middle, that is, performance was significantly lowered (sessions 15–17: 0.75 vs. sessions 3–5: 0.80;
p < 0.001). Thus the interference seems to be even for remembering the learned mean discrimination skill when performing a different task, outlier detection, in the middle.
Figure 6 bottom shows performance RT for mean discrimination. Performance, and even learning, after the switch is slower than initial learning, reflecting the interference found earlier (
t-test: mean discrimination RT: sessions 1–5: 624 ms vs. sessions 6–10: 720 ms;
p < 0.001;) However, initial RT speeding due to initial training is fully maintained after back and forth switch (
t-test: sessions 3–5: 548 ms vs. sessions 15–17: 543 ms;
p = 0.38 n.s.).
The differences in development and transfer measured by accuracy versus by RT suggests that there are different processes that determine these two performance parameters. Perhaps RT improvement is generalized across tasks, whereas accuracy requires specific learning, and presence of a confusing parameter leads to its being ignored or inhibited.
Figure 7 top shows performance of outlier detection for the first five sessions (average accuracy 0.73) and last three sessions of the group who performed outlier detection first (and last), as well as the outlier detection performance by the other group, who performed this task in their sixth to 14th sessions. There is little to no difference in performance when comparing performance after switching (0.72) with initial performance (0.73). In other words, there is little if any advantage of having performed mean discrimination for five sessions, even with presence of the outlier (session 1–5 vs. 6–10:
t-test:
p = 0.31). This may be interpreted in two ways: either there is little improvement and little interference from mean discrimination to outlier detection, or there may be both some learning and some interference and they largely cancel each other out. Looking at performance for the last three sessions, after switching back and forth (0.71), there is carryover of performance improvement from the original training (sessions 3–5;
p = 0.12). Performance of the mean discrimination task in the middle does not interfere much with outlier detection performance.
Figure 7 bottom shows performance RT for outlier detection. Learning after the switch (sessions 6–10: 692 ms) is faster than initial learning (sessions 1–5: 727 ms:
p < 0.01), and initial learning is fully maintained after back and forth switch (sessions 3–5: 709 ms vs. sessions 15–17: 657 ms;
p < 0.05), although five sessions may not have sufficed for full training. Again, the differences in accuracy versus RT measures suggests that RT is generalized, whereas accuracy is specific.
These results suggest that performance with an implicit second task might be like performance without the second task present at all. However, presence of the second implicit task seems to prevent perceptual learning transfer from task to task, suggesting it is suppressed when present but not performed. Furthermore, although second task presence leaves learning of the original task undisrupted, its performance is disturbed by switching tasks back and forth.