Figure 2A shows behavioral performance as a function of experimental condition (collapsed over distractor type, because this did not yield consistent results, as discussed elsewhere in this article). To analyze the effects of item and feature-dimension retro-cues, we quantified retro-cueing benefits as the difference between the trials with informative and neutral retro-cues (
Figure 2B).
To formally quantify the effects of retro-cue informativeness (valid or neutral) and retro-cue block type (item retro-cue block or dimension retro-cue block) we used a 2 × 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance. We ran this separately for response time and response error, and separately for both color and orientation recall reports. We observed a significant main effect of retro-cue informativeness, with better performance following informative versus neutral retro-cues on all four dependent variables: orientation error, F(1, 29) = 18.888, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.394; color error, F(1, 29) = 26.387, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.476; orientation response time, F(1, 29) = 67.27, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.699; and color response time , F(1, 29) = 65.15, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.692. At the same time, we found that the behavioral benefits of retro-cue informativeness were larger in item retro-cue block than in dimension retro-cue block, yielding a significant interaction for color error, F(1, 29) = 9.065, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.238; orientation response time, F(1, 29) = 19.64, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.404; and color response time, F(1, 29) = 21.00, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.420. Although we found the same trend, this did not reach significance for orientation error, F(1, 29) = 3.750, p = 0.063, η2 = 0.115. Finally, in line with the greater benefit of item retro-cues, we also found a significant main effect of block type, constituted by better performance in item retro-cue blocks for all four dependent variables: orientation error, F(1, 29) = 39.634, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.577; color error, F1,29 = 8.343, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.223; orientation response time, F1,29 = 50.51, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.635; and color response time, F(1, 29) = 32.08, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.525.
We also considered the third factor, namely, distractor congruence (i.e., when the distractor contained the same or the other feature dimension as the to-be-recalled memory dimension), but found no systematic effects of distractor congruence across our four dependent variables, nor interactions with the factors of interest (
Supplementary Table 1).
We describe in greater detail the item and dimension retro-cueing effects of interest, in accordance with the data presented in
Figure 2.
For orientation recall reports, participants significantly benefitted from item retro-cues. They had smaller errors, t29 = 4.235, p < 0.001, d = 0.773, and responded faster, t29 = 7.854, p < 0.001, d = 1.434, compared with trials with neutral retro-cues in the same block types. Similarly, orientation reports benefitted significantly from dimension cues in both reproduction error, t29 = 3.748, p = 0.001, d = 0.684, and response onset time, t29 = 6.302, p < 0.001, d = 1.151, compared with neutral trials within the dimension retro-cueing blocks. Item cues conferred numerically larger benefits than dimension cues. The difference was not statistically significant for error, 0.023 rad., 48%, t29 = 1.936, p = 0.063, d = .354, but reached significance for reaction times, 79 ms, 94%, t29 = 4.431, p < 0.001; d = 0.809.
The same pattern of results was found for the error and reaction times in the color recall trials: color reports benefitted from both item cues, t29 = 5.060, p < 0.001, d = 0.924, and dimension cues, t29 = 4.069, p < 0.001, d = 0.743, and responses were also faster for item cues, t29 = 9.097, p < 0.001, d = 1.661, and dimension cues, t29 = 4.951, p < 0.001, d = 0.904, compared with their respective neutral trials. For color reports, we also found greater benefits of item retro-cues compared with dimension retro-cues for both error, 0.039 rad., 86%, t29 = 3.011, p = 0.005, d = 0.550, and reaction time, 63 ms, 91%, t29 = 4.583, p < 0.001, d = 0.837.
The benefits of item-based and dimension-based retro-cueing showed strong positive correlations across individuals for both color and orientation reports (
Figure 2C). For orientation reports, we found significant correlations between retro-cueing benefits following item cues and dimension cues for both error,
r = .709,
p < 0.001, and reaction time,
r = .539,
p = 0.002,. Likewise, for color reports, we found significant correlations between retro-cueing benefits following item cues and dimension cues for both error,
r = .697,
p < 0.001, and reaction time,
r = .596,
p < 0.001. Thus, participants who benefitted most from item retro-cues also benefitted most from dimension retro-cues.