Figure 10 reveals an unusual pattern of results. The conjunction search is easier (or, at the very least, certainly not harder) than the component feature searches. As in
Experiment 1, we found that slopes of the RT × set size functions were quite efficient for segmentable and non-segmentable conjunction search tasks (6-7 ms/item in target-present and 13-17 ms/item in the target-absent trials). For color feature search, the slopes were also reasonably efficient search (8 ms/item in the target-present and 21 ms/item in the target-absent trials). For orientation feature search, the slopes were inefficient (21 ms/item in the target-present and 53 ms/item in the target-absent trials.
For target-present trials, we found the strong effect of the search task on the slope (
F[3, 39] = 12.74,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.50, BF
10 = 1.07 × 10
4). Pairwise comparisons showed that slopes in the orientation search task were much steeper than in other conditions (
ts[13] > 3.57,
ps ≤ 0.003, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's
ds > 0.95, BF
10s > 13.79,
Figure 10). The color feature search did not differ from the conjunction tasks (
ts[13] < 0.8,
ps > .438, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's
ds < 0.215, BF
10′s < 0.357). For target-absent trials, the pattern was the same (
F (3, 39) = 15.98,
p < 0.001, η
2 = 0.55, BF
10 = 6.85×10
4), with the orientation search task producing much greater slopes than other three tasks (
ts[13] > 4.10,
ps < 0.0011, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's
ds > 1.1, BF
10′s > 32.53,
Figure 10) with the rest of conditions not differing from each other (
ts[13] < 2.07,
ps > .06, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's
ds < 0.55, BF
10′s < 1.37).
We also found similar effects of the task on the average RTs in target-present trials (F[3, 39] = 16.77, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.56, BF10 = 7.92 × 104). Again, the main effect was driven by the orientation search task yielding a greater average RT than the two conjunction searches (ts[13] > 6.38, ps < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected α = 0 .008, Cohen's ds > 1.70, BF10s > 1.01 × 103), whereas other conditions did not differ from each other (ts[13] < 2.97, ps > .01, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds < 0.79, BF10′s < 5.23). For target-absent trials, the main effect was also present (F[3, 39] = 22.08, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.63, BF10 = 1.82 × 106). The orientation search average RT was bigger than the RTs in the rest of the conditions (ts[13] = 4.50, ps < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds = 1.20, BF10s = 195.43), while the rest of the tasks did not differ (ts[13] < 2.96, ps > 0.01, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds < 0.79, BF10s < 5.23).
Finally, we found that the different tasks produced different error rates in both target-present (F[3, 39] = 58.6, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.82, BF10 = 1.48 × 1013) and target-absent (F[3, 39] = 16.71, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.56, BF10 = 2.3 × 105) trials. In target-present trials, this effect was basically provided by the orientation search with an average of 27% errors (“miss” responses) that was greater than in the rest of the tasks with an average of 6% to 11% misses (ts[13] > 6.65, ps < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds > 1.78, BF10s > 1478). We also found that segmentable conjunction search yielded a slightly bigger rate of miss errors than the color search (t[13] = 3.71, p = 0.004, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's d = 0.99, BF10 = 17.259). The rest of the conditions did not differ from each other in terms of the miss errors (ts[13] < 2.62, ps > 0.02, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds < 0.7, BF10s < 3.08). For target-absent trials, the pattern of errors (“false” alarms) was different. Here, we found that the color search task produced ∼12% false alarms on average; a rate that was higher than in the other tasks (∼2%–5% false alarms, t[13] > 3.62, p < 0.004, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's d > 0.97, BF10 > 14.87), whereas the remaining conditions did not differ from each other (ts[13] < 2.54, ps > 0.02, Bonferroni corrected α = 0.008, Cohen's ds < 0.68, BF10s < 2.75).
In
Experiment 4, we replicated the finding from
Experiment 1 that conjunction search can be quite efficient and is not affected by the segmentability of distractors. The novel aspect of the
Experiment 4 results is the finding that the non-segmentable feature searches were harder than the conjunction searches. In the case of the orientation search, the feature searches were markedly less efficient. The feature search is more sensitive to non-segmentability than conjunction search because conjunction search can make better use of feature guidance than the feature searches in this case. The combination of two relatively noisy feature activation maps produces a combined priority map that can do quite a good job of guiding attention to a conjunction target (as suggested by the model in
Figure 6). Therefore our results provide evidence that feature grouping and segmentation, as well as heterogeneity, in general, are important determinants of search efficiency, but top-down guidance can be powerful enough to overcome their effects on search. The detrimental effect of poor segmentability, observed in this experiment, may seem opposite to the facilitating effect found in
Utochkin and Yurevich (2016). However, both effects are flip sides of the same mechanism. In
Utochkin and Yurevich (2016), the target was always highly distinct from all distractors (see the example in
Figure 1 of the present article), so non-segmentable distractors provided stronger background grouping and better target segmentation. In the present experiment, the target feature was always a part of the non-segmentable group, so it was harder to detect it.
At first glance, the fact that feature search is harder than conjunction search is at odds with one of the standard findings of visual search literature (
Treisman & Gelade, 1980). However, if one accepts that conjunction search can be guided by several features simultaneously, this result is showing that search becomes easier as the difference between the target and distractors increases. As is obvious from
Figure 11, the non-segmentable feature search conditions in
Experiment 4 involve some very small target-distractor (TD) distances within either feature space alone. In contrast, because color and orientation of distractors are negatively correlated in the non-segmentable conjunction case, a small color TD difference will be accompanied by a large orientation difference and vice versa. In a standard segmentable situation, the TD differences in the feature search tasks would be large, resulting in very efficient feature search with slopes near 0. This would be more efficient than guided conjunction search.