One purpose of our study was to test a potential explanation for the common experience that more light makes better sight, an observation that likely predates written history when cave-dwelling artists painted ancient figures by torchlight. All of the classic scientific reports from the 18th to 20th centuries reviewed in
Figure 1 emphasized a large, monotonic increase of acuity with retinal illuminance. To account for this phenomenon, we hypothesized that, when retinal illumination is reduced below the foveal cone threshold, observers adopt a strategy of manipulating gaze (or attention) to observe the stimulus when located at the optimum retinal locus for maximizing acuity, given the available level of retinal illumination. By this account, the classical free-viewing paradigm creates a tension between the need for visibility (by
increasing eccentricity to increase rod density) and the need for legibility (by
decreasing eccentricity in order to increase neural sampling density). To test this idea, we measured the effect of retinal illuminance on visual acuity under scotopic and mesopic conditions while fixing gaze to allow controlled placement of the stimulus image at known retinal locations. Our results (
Figure 9) are compared in
Figure 10 with those from classical studies (
Figure 1). For this purpose we excluded the data from
Mayer (1755) and from
Koenig (1897) because of our concern that conversion of antiquated photometric units that depended on assumptions regarding pupil size may have produced erroneous estimates of retinal illuminance. We also excluded the abnormally high acuity values inferred from contrast sensitivity functions published by
Van Nes and Bouman (1967) on the grounds that their observers probably used a detection criterion rather than a resolution criterion. As shown in
Figure 8 and described previously (
Thibos, Still, & Bradley, 1996), detection acuity is typically greater than resolution acuity in peripheral vision. Thus, we concentrate our attention in
Figure 10 on acuity values reported by
Shlaer (1937) and by
van Meeteren and Vos (1972), which are nearly identical. We justify the direct comparison of acuity obtained with interference fringes (which do not suffer contrast attenuation by optical aberrations of the eye) with natural viewing of conventional stimuli (without optical correction of refractive errors) on the grounds that resolution acuity for sinusoidal gratings is sampling limited, not contrast limited. Even large focusing errors have no effect on peripheral resolution acuity for high-contrast stimuli (
Anderson, 1996;
Wang, Thibos, & Bradley, 1997).