SSD and the blanking effect are robust phenomena that have been studied extensively in adults; however, we do not know whether children also experience these effects or when the potential mechanisms underlying these effects develop. Basic visual perception develops within the first few years of age (
Braddick & Atkinson, 2011), but it is unclear when transsaccadic perceptual processes develop in children; their development may rely more on the development of the oculomotor system than on the development of visual perception and may also reflect the greater variability in saccade execution that accompanies this development. The saccade planning system seems to still be developing up until around 8 years of age. Saccade latencies decrease with age (
Bucci & Seassau, 2012;
Cohen & Ross, 1977;
Munoz et al., 1998;
Salman et al., 2006) and reach the same level as adults by the age of 12 (
Fukushima, Hatta, & Fukushima, 2000). Saccade gain seems to increase with age (
Bucci & Seassau, 2012), with some studies indicating that children reach adult-like performance by the age of 8 (
Munoz et al., 1998;
Salman et al., 2006). This developing oculomotor control may result in greater uncertainty in saccade planning or execution. The models of
Atsma et al. (2016) and
Niemeier et al. (2003) would predict that this increased uncertainty may lead to greater SSD. Indeed, this seems to be the case for one such transsaccadic perceptual phenomenon, as saccadic suppression of contrast sensitivity is even more pronounced in children than in adults, with children showing three times more suppression than adults (
Bruno, Brambati, Perani, & Morrone, 2006). This could be due to a stronger need to suppress information due to uncertainty in developing oculomotor functions (
Bruno et al., 2006;
Niemeier et al., 2003). If the development of SSD is similar to saccadic suppression of contrast sensitivity, then we would expect to see stronger SSD in children than adults. Studies on the development of multisensory integration may also give us insight as to when transsaccadic integrative processes may develop, because the integration of transsaccadic information and multisensory integration have been shown to rely on the same principles of optimal cue combination (
Ganmor et al., 2015;
Wolf & Schutz, 2015). Transsaccadic integration has not been studied in children; however, studies into the development of multisensory information suggest a rather late development.
Nardini et al. (2008) showed that, while adults could optimally integrate and weight landmark and non-visual self-motion cues in a navigation task, children between 4 and 8 years of age failed to integrate the cues. Similarly, the integration of visuohaptic information develops only after the age of 8 to 10, before which children rely on a single modality (
Gori et al., 2008). This late development may be due to the ongoing process of calibration to account for perceptual and sensorimotor development, or a failure to develop correspondence between different signals (
Ernst, 2008;
Gori et al., 2008). This latter idea was supported by
Jovanovic & Drewing (2014), who found that children 6 years of age can integrate visuohaptic information, but only when the discrepancy between the stimuli was small, and when the stimuli were more likely to be attributed to a single origin. Calibration of perceptual and sensorimotor processes and causal correspondence are both elements that may play a role in integrating pre- and postsaccadic position information (
Atsma et al., 2016;
Niemeier et al., 2003). The processes underlying transsaccadic information integration may also still be subject to a sensorimotor and perceptual calibration process in children. Integration of information during SSD may reflect integration processes involved in multisensory integration, in which case we would expect to see less integration of information and therefore less SSD in children than adults.