Presaccadic information encoded in memory has long been known to affect postsaccadic performance, for example, providing preview benefits in object naming (
Pollatsek, Rayner, & Collins, 1984;
Henderson, Pollatsek, & Rayner, 1987) and letter identification (
Henderson & Anes, 1994). This preview effect is reminiscent of even earlier work that showed that a peripheral preview facilitates foveal recognition, even without a saccade (see footnote 3,
Wagner, 1918). Transsaccadic priming seemed to work on both a more abstract level, for example, an object or category name, or on a more precise level dependent on the features of the objects (
Pollatsek et al., 1984), and this research paved the way for the idea that specific object features could be retained in memory across saccades to facilitate postsaccadic perception. Unlike these earlier transsaccadic priming studies, which focussed mainly on how presaccadic information facilitates speeded object identification responses, the newer wave of transsaccadic feature integration studies have a greater focus on how the presaccadic, peripherally viewed percept of a feature directly interacts, or is combined with, the postsaccadic, foveal percept of that same feature. In the previous section, we discussed cases where the effects of the presaccadic stimulus were immediately measurable in the postsaccadic percept (i.e.
Fabius et al., 2016). In this section, we will focus on a different subsection of studies into transsaccadic feature integration, where pre- and postsaccadic stimuli are treated as more discrete elements, and where feature information from both are extracted, encoded into memory, and subsequently combined to form a composite, averaged percept (see
Figure 5). When this averaging occurs, the postsaccadic percept is biased toward the presaccadic percept. For example, the shape of a presaccadically presented ellipse will result in a bias toward the perceived shape of the postsaccadic ellipse, such that the reported percept is an average of the pre- and postsaccadic shapes (
Demeyer, Graef, Wagemans, & Verfaillie, 2010). Similarly, the perceived color of a color patch presented before the saccade will be shifted toward the color of a postsaccadic stimulus presented in the same spatial location (
Wittenberg, Bremmer, & Wachtler, 2008). The weighting with which pre- and postsaccadic information is combined is not, however, equal, rather it occurs in proportion to the relative reliability of each stimulus. When the contrast of incongruent pre- and postsaccadic shapes is varied, the accuracy of identifying the postsaccadic shape depends on both the contrast and congruency of pre- and postsaccadic stimuli (
Demeyer, Graef, Wagemans, & Verfaillie, 2009); when the level of noise present in the pre- and postsaccadic stimuli is varied, more weight is given to the more reliable signal, so that the perceived color is biased toward the less noisy percept (
Oostwoud Wijdenes, Marshall, & Bays, 2015). As with many other interactions, the fovea also naturally dominates this weighting, given the higher reliability of foveal vision: indeed, when the reliabilities of the pre- and postsaccadic percepts are not equated by experimental manipulation, the percept is dominated by the postsaccadic stimulus (
Wolf & Schütz, 2015;
Schut, Stoep, der, Fabius, & Van der Stigchel, 2018). While in natural vision it is most likely that the reliability of the postsaccadic percept will be higher than the postsaccadic percept, the weighting mechanism demonstrates that the visual system does not simply assume that the foveal percept will have higher reliability, but is able to flexibly change the weighting of the percepts based on the current input.