Open Access
Article  |   December 2020
Motion-form interaction: Motion and form aftereffects induced by distorted static natural scenes
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Footnotes
    *  KR and SWH contributed equally to the study.
Journal of Vision December 2020, Vol.20, 10. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.13.10
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Katharina Rifai, Selam W. Habtegiorgis, Caroline Erlenwein, Siegfried Wahl; Motion-form interaction: Motion and form aftereffects induced by distorted static natural scenes. Journal of Vision 2020;20(13):10. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.13.10.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Spatially varying distortions (SVDs) are common artifacts of spectacles like progressive additional lenses (PALs). To habituate to distortions of PALs, the visual system has to adapt to distortion-induced image alterations, termed skew adaptation. But how this visual adjustment is achieved is largely unknown. This study examines the properties of visual adaptation to distortions of PALs in natural scenes. The visual adaptation in response to altered form and motion features of the natural stimuli were probed in two different psychophysical experiments. Observers were exposed to distortions in natural images, and form and motion aftereffects were tested subsequently in a constant stimuli procedure where subjects were asked to judge the skew, or the motion direction of an according test stimulus.

Exposure to skewed natural stimuli induced a shift in perceived undistorted form as well as motion direction, when viewing distorted dynamic natural scenes, and also after exposure to static distorted natural images. Therefore, skew adaptation occurred in form and motion for dynamic visual scenes as well as static images. Thus, specifically in the condition of static skewed images and the test feature of motion direction, cortical interactions between motion-form processing presumably contributed to the adaptation process.

In a nutshell, interfeature cortical interactions constituted the adaptation process to distortion of PALs. Thus, comprehensive investigation of adaptation to distortions of PALs would benefit from taking into account content richness of the stimuli to be used, like natural images.

Introduction
Progressive additional lenses (PALs) are commonly used optical elements for correction of presbyopia. They combine refractive corrections with an area of increased power in the lower area of the lens to support near vision in presbyopes. To avoid the disturbing optical inhomogeneity of bifocal spectacles, PALs have a continuous meridional power change (Sheedy, Campbell, King-Smith, & Hayes, 2005). As a consequence, a typical artifact of PALs are spatial geometrical distortions, the most prominent distortion of the lens being skew, a horizontally and vertically equal scale image shear (Habtegiorgis, Rifai, Lappe, & Wahl, 2017a; Meister & Fisher, 2008). The degree of distortion varies over the lens surface, showing an individual fingerprint depending on refractive error and lens design. In all PALs, distortion is most prominent in peripheral areas of such lenses (Barbero & Portilla, 2015). Optical distortions alter a variety of features of natural image content, such as position information, form, spatial frequency, luminance, contrast, orientation, texture, or optic flow signals (Bex & Makous, 2002; Bex, Mareschal, & Dakin, 2007; Bex, Solomon, & Dakin, 2009; Billock, De Guzman, & Scott Kelso, 2001; Dong & Atick, 1995; Heath, McCormack, & Vaughan, 1987). Through the dependency of optical distortions on position of the lens, in novel PAL wearers, objects appear distorted primarily when looking through peripheral parts of the lens (Gibson, 1937; Gibson & Radner, 1937). Upon head movement and gaze changes in the lens, artificial motion perception occurs (Kohler, 1962; Meister & Fisher, 2008; Welch, 1974; Welch, Carterette, & Friedman, 1978). However, after prolonged use, wearers report vanishing of side effects (Alvarez, Kim, & Granger-Donetti, 2017; Fonda, 1980; Kohler, 1962; Pick & Hay, 1964; Welch, 1974). Hence, habituation to PALs in the form of visual adaptation is hypothesized to contribute to restore undistorted vision. Also, other features, such as magnification, induce visual adaptation (Epstein, 1972; Kinney, Luria, & Weitzman, 1968; Ross, 1979; Vlaskamp, Filippini, & Banks, 2009). Visual adaptation is a process by which the visual system modifies its operating properties in response to changes in features (Clifford et al., 2007; Habtegiorgis, Rifai, Lappe, & Wahl, 2017b). How exactly the visual system adapts to distortion-induced alterations in the natural visual world is largely unknown. In PAL distortions specifically, due to the coexistence of prominent alterations of form and motion features, the contribution of adaptation in the respective features is unclear. 
Distortion-induced alteration (e.g., in form and motion features) possibly leads to plasticity in feature-selective neurons in the visual system. Livingstone and Hubel (1987) determined that there were two different and parallel working pathways in the visual system for form and motion information processing. However, neurophysiological and psychophysical studies provided evidence for the interaction between form and motion pathways (Barlow & Olshausen, 2004; Edwards & Crane, 2007; Fu, Shen, Gao, & Dan, 2004; Geisler, 1999; Geisler, Albrecht, Crane, & Stern, 2001; Grossberg, 1991; Mather, Pavan, Bellacosa, & Casco, 2012). Thus, understanding visual adaptation to PAL distortions in a natural environment requires the knowledge of distortion-induced plasticity in the motion and form visual pathways and their interaction. 
The present study uses short-term adaptation to skew distorted natural image content as a model system for visual adaptation as a habituation mechanism to PALs. Specifically, both form and motion adaptation were probed by appropriate test stimuli in response to dynamic as well as static distorted content. In the first experiment, form adaptation in natural images was compared in the presence and absence of motion information. Form adaptation still existed when motion information was removed. The second experiment evaluated adaptation of motion direction after exposure to distorted natural images. Motion adaptation was measured in the same conditions as in Experiment 1. Motion adaptation was induced by distorted natural image sequences and also after exposure to static distorted photographs. Hence, this study revealed unknown interfeature interaction between form and motion features during skew adaptation in natural scenes. 
Experiment 1: Form adaptation aftereffects induced by distorted form features of natural scenes
Habtegiorgis and colleagues (2017b) previously showed a form adaptation aftereffect (FAE) induced by distorted natural scenes. The aim of the present experiment was to examine whether form adaptation still exists when motion information is removed from such stimuli. Thus, form adaptation was measured in the presence and absence of motion information in two separate conditions: FAE after exposure to dynamic skewed natural image sequences (Condition 1) and FAE after exposure to static skewed natural images (Condition 2). In Condition 1, the FAE induced by both form and motion statistics of skewed natural image content represented a baseline to replicate previous results (Habtegiorgis et al., 2017b). In Condition 2, it was tested if FAE occurs in response to skewed natural image content and thus by form statistics only. 
Materials and methods
Study approval
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen and the University Hospital. 
Observers
Ten observers participated in this psychophysical experiment. Observers had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All observers were naive about the objective of the experiment. In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, participants gave their informed written consent before participating in the experiment. 
Experimental setup
The study was designed in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) using Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (Brainard, 1997) on an apple computer (Apple, Cupertino, CA, USA). During the experiment, the stimuli were presented on a CRT monitor (HM204DT A; Iiyama) with a screen resolution of 1,280 × 1,024 pixels (406 × 304 mm). The screen refresh rate was 85 Hz. 
A chin- and headrest was used to stabilize the head of observers. The viewing distance was 60 cm to the screen. The whole screen subtended a visual angle (VA) of 37° horizontally and 28° vertically. The stimuli were presented at 0° eccentricity and subtended a visual angle of 20° both horizontally and vertically. The experiment was conducted in an entirely darkened room. Right or left keys of a keyboard were used to collect observers’ responses after adaptation. 
Stimuli
Images were skewed by a shear angle of θ by remapping pixel positions x and y to distorted positions xd and yd using geometrical skew transformation matrix M in Equations 1 and 2 (Habtegiorgis et al., 2017b).  
\begin{equation}\left( {\begin{array}{@{}*{1}{c}@{}} {{x_d}}\\ {{y_d}} \end{array}} \right) = M{\rm{*\;}}\left( {\begin{array}{@{}*{1}{c}@{}} x\\ y \end{array}} \right).\end{equation}
(1)
 
\begin{equation}M = {\rm{\;}}\left( {\begin{array}{@{}*{1}{c}@{}} {1{\rm{\;}}tan\theta }\\ {tan\theta \;{\rm{\;}}1{\rm{\;}}} \end{array}{\rm{\;}}} \right).\end{equation}
(2)
 
In each measurement condition, adapting stimuli were taken from two groups of skewed natural images, containing left-skewed images with a skew angle of θ =   − 25° and right-skewed images with a skew angle of θ =   + 25°. The image content was identical in the oppositely skewed adapting stimuli groups. Only the inner 650 × 650 pixels of each skewed image were used. Furthermore, a Hanning window weighting function of the second order was applied to the skewed image to blend out the sharp boundaries (Harris, 1978). The window was used as a weighting function and had the same size as the images (i.e., 650 × 650 pixels). The distorted and blended images subtended a visual angle of 20° × 20° in the horizontal and the vertical direction at zero eccentricity. 
In Condition 1, adapting stimuli contained temporally correlated skewed natural image sequences. In total, 27,000 sequential frames from three different open-source movies were skewed and applied as adapting stimuli (Baumann, 2010; Kluge, 2008; Roosendaal, 2012). These image sequences were rendered at 30 frames per second. Through the temporal correlation of frames, not only each image but also motion statistics were strongly influenced by the skew. A description of the resulting motion statistics can be found in Habtegiorgis, Jarvers, Rifai, Neumann, and Wahl (2019a). 
In Condition 2, adapting stimuli were static skewed natural images. A total of 130 photographs containing different natural scenes (e.g., buildings, animals, flowers, or people) were taken from the McGill-calibrated color image database and Olmos and Kingdom (2004). In this condition, image content differs frame by frame; thus, stimuli are temporally uncorrelated, and motion statistics is unaffected by the skew. 
Form adaptation aftereffects were tested with plaid checkerboards of the same size as the adapting stimuli (Habtegiorgis et al., 2017b). The plaid was constructed by superimposing identical contrast sinusoidal gratings orientated at –45° to the right and +45° to the left. The spatial wavelength of the component gratings corresponded to the dimension of the squares’ diagonals in the plaid. The unskewed diagonals of the checkerboard have a horizontal and vertical dimension of d0 = 40 pixels subtending a VA of 1.24°. Different skew amplitudes were induced in the plaid by varying the wavelength of the component gratings. 
The dimensions of the plaids' opposite diagonals were defined by the wavelengths of the component gratings in the corresponding directions (i.e., dright and dleft) and had the same value d0 = 1.24° of VA for a square plaid. Skew was induced by adding or subtracting Δ from the wavelengths the component gratings, as in Equation 3. Nonzero Δ stretches the plaid diagonally and shears the zero-crossings of the squares in the plaid.  
\begin{equation}\begin{array}{@{}l@{}} {d_{right}} = \;{d_0} - \;\Delta \\ {d_{left}} = \;{d_0} + \;\Delta \end{array}\end{equation}
(3)
 
Thus, plaids are skewed to the left when Δ is positive and to the right when Δ is negative. The skew amplitude Askew was quantified by the magnification in oblique directions, which was induced by either geometrically shearing or varying the diagonal dimensions, as presented in Equation 4.  
\begin{eqnarray}{A_{skew}}\left( {\Delta /\theta } \right) &=& 1 - \;\frac{{{d_{right}}}}{{{d_{left}}}} = 1 - \;\frac{{{d_0} - \Delta }}{{{d_0} + \;\Delta }} \nonumber\\ &=& 1 - \;\frac{{1 - \;\tan \theta }}{{1 + \tan \theta }}.\;\end{eqnarray}
(4)
 
Examples from the two groups of adapting stimuli together with test stimuli are shown in Figure 1
Figure 1.
 
(a) Example of a skewed natural image sequence used as adaptor in Condition 1 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Image skew is 25° and –25°, respectively. (b) Example of 25°/–25° skewed natural image used as adaptor in Condition 2 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Additional static images are shown in Supplementary Figure S1. (c) Example of plaid checkerboard test stimuli with left and right skew directions of –14.5% and 14.5%.
Figure 1.
 
(a) Example of a skewed natural image sequence used as adaptor in Condition 1 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Image skew is 25° and –25°, respectively. (b) Example of 25°/–25° skewed natural image used as adaptor in Condition 2 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Additional static images are shown in Supplementary Figure S1. (c) Example of plaid checkerboard test stimuli with left and right skew directions of –14.5% and 14.5%.
Procedure
Observers in this study were informed about the procedure and instructed on how to respond to a test stimulus with a keyboard before experiment start. Viewing was monocular with the right eye. Adaptation was tested alternately to oppositely skewed stimuli successively, first to left-skewed and then to right-skewed natural image sequences. Adaptation aftereffect was tested after each adaptation using the method of constant stimuli. Skew amplitudes ranging from –14.5% to 14.5% were used as test stimuli in 10 discrete equidistant steps with 10 repetitions of each test stimulus in random order. Overall, 100 responses were recorded per subject to compute the psychometric curve of each adapting skew direction. For each condition, the same psychophysical procedure was followed, except that the adapting stimuli differed in the two conditions. At first, adaptation was induced by a 3-s exposure to the adaptation stimulus. In Condition 1, the dynamic image sequence was applied, whereas a single image was shown in Condition 2. Next, one of the test stimuli was presented for 0.25 s. Then, observers responded whether the checkerboard was skewed to the left or right by pressing the left or right key on the keyboard, respectively. The sequence of adaptation exposure, test stimulus, and response phase was repeated, alternating for the two skew directions. Experimental sessions of Condition 1 and Condition 2 were separated by a minimum of 24 hr. 
Statistical data analysis
The percentage of rightward responses was computed at each test stimulus skew amplitude. A cumulative Gaussian function was used to fit the percentage of rightward responses as a function of skew amplitude of the test stimuli with asymptotes set free but assumed to be equal (Schütt, Harmeling, Macke, & Wichmann, 2016). The point of subjective equality (PSE; i.e., the skew amplitude that was perceived to be unskewed) was defined by the skew amplitude at 50% of rightward responses. The adaptation aftereffect was evaluated by the difference between the PSE of the left- and right-skew adaptation (ΔPSE). The ΔPSEs from all observers were averaged to estimate the overall adaptation aftereffect. The significance of perceptual shift was determined by applying a paired t test on the ΔPSEs. 
Results
Figure 2 shows psychometric functions of the average response of all observers measured in the two conditions. The percentage of rightward responses was computed as a function of test stimuli skew amplitude. Negative skew amplitudes corresponded to left-skewed plaid checkerboards, and positive skew amplitudes corresponded to right-skewed plaid checkerboards. The graphs show data together with confidence intervals at PSE in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffect and in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffect. 
Figure 2.
 
FAE from all the observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows data, sigmoid fits, and confidence intervals at PSE in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Negative stimulus skew amplitudes refer to left-skewed stimuli, and positive skew amplitudes refer to right-skewed stimuli. The bar plots in the right column show the average ΔPSE of all observers. The error bars show the standard error.
Figure 2.
 
FAE from all the observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows data, sigmoid fits, and confidence intervals at PSE in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Negative stimulus skew amplitudes refer to left-skewed stimuli, and positive skew amplitudes refer to right-skewed stimuli. The bar plots in the right column show the average ΔPSE of all observers. The error bars show the standard error.
In both conditions, the PSE shifted toward the direction of skew after left- and right-skew adaptation. After exposure to left-skewed stimuli, observers perceived a left-skewed plaid checkerboard as unskewed and vice versa. In the left column of Figure 2, the average ΔPSEs over all the observers are shown. ΔPSE of Condition 1 was 7.18% ± 1.69%; ΔPSE of Condition 2 was 7.87% ± 1.37%. The effect sizes of the ΔPSEs measured in Conditions 1 and 2 were significantly different from zero, p < 0.05. 
In sum, adaptation aftereffects were observed within both conditions. Moreover, stimuli with only form content induced a similar FAE as stimuli containing form and motion features. 
Experiment 2: Motion adaptation aftereffects induced by distorted form features of natural scenes
Materials and methods
The materials and methods used in this experiment were the same as in Experiment 1, except for modifications described below. Motion content of the adaptation stimulus has been thoroughly described in Habtegiorgis et al. (2019a). 
Study approval
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen and the University Hospital. 
Observers
The same participants as in Experiment 1 participated in this psychophysical experiment. Participants gave their informed written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki prior to the experiment. 
Test stimuli
In both measurement conditions, test stimuli were dynamic random dots. Each dot was circular and subtended a VA of 0.14°. The annulus in which the dots were moving subtended a VA of 15° × 15° and always consists of 2,000 dots. In the center of the annulus, a red point was presented for central fixation. The dots moved coherently at a speed of 4°/s. The direction of the coherent motion was diagonally up or down at an angle of θtilt from the horizontal. For a specific motion direction, θtilt, the position of the dots x1 and y1 was updated to x2 and y2 in the subsequent frame using Equation 5. Positive θtilt corresponded to upward motion and negative to downward motion. The test stimulus to measure motion aftereffects is presented in Figure 3.  
\begin{equation}\frac{{{x_2}}}{{{y_2}}} = \;\frac{{\cos \theta \cdot {x_1}}}{{\sin \theta \cdot {y_2}}}.\;\end{equation}
(5)
 
Figure 3.
 
Dynamic random dot test stimuli for measurement of motion direction aftereffects. Illustration of (a) upward and (b) downward motion direction.
Figure 3.
 
Dynamic random dot test stimuli for measurement of motion direction aftereffects. Illustration of (a) upward and (b) downward motion direction.
Procedure
In this experiment, the test stimulus was presented with 12 different motion directions, ranging from –6.6° to 6.6° in steps of 1.2°. Each test stimulus was repeated 10 times. Thus, 120 responses were recorded to fit the psychometric function of each adapting skew direction. Observers responded whether the dots were moving up or down by pressing the up or down keys on the keyboard, respectively. The order of the test stimuli of different motion directions was randomized. Experimental sessions of Condition 1 with skewed image sequences and Condition 2 with skewed static images as adaptors were separated by a minimum of 24 hr. 
Statistical data analysis
The percentage of upward responses was fitted as a function of test motion direction with the asymptotes set free but assumed to be equal (Schütt et al., 2016). The PSE (i.e., the motion direction that was perceived as horizontal) was defined by the motion direction at 50% of upward responses. The adaptation aftereffect was evaluated by the difference between the PSE of the left- and right-skew adaptation (ΔPSE). The ΔPSEs from all observers were averaged to estimate the overall adaptation aftereffect. The significance of perceptual shift was determined by applying a paired t test on the ΔPSEs. 
Results
Figure 4 shows the psychometric fits of the average response over all observers, separately for the two conditions. The percentage of upward responses is shown as a function of test stimulus direction. Negative motion directions correspond to downward motion, and positive motion directions correspond to upward motion. 
Figure 4.
 
MAE from all observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows psychometric curves of the average response of all the observers. Sigmoid fit, data, and confidence intervals at PSE are shown in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Positive θtilt corresponded to upward motion and negative to downward motion. In the second column, bar plots show the average ΔPSE of all the observers. The error bars show the standard error.
Figure 4.
 
MAE from all observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows psychometric curves of the average response of all the observers. Sigmoid fit, data, and confidence intervals at PSE are shown in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Positive θtilt corresponded to upward motion and negative to downward motion. In the second column, bar plots show the average ΔPSE of all the observers. The error bars show the standard error.
In both Condition 1 containing image sequences and Condition 2 containing static images as adaptors, the PSE shifted toward the adapting skew direction. Observers perceived an upward motion as a horizontal movement after adaptation by right-skewed images and vice versa. ΔPSE of Condition 1 was 2.78° ± 0.77°, and ΔPSE of Condition 2 was 4.49° ± 1.46°. The ΔPSEs from all observers were significantly different from zero in both conditions, t test <0.05. The corresponding averages are shown in the second column of Figure 4
Discussion
This study demonstrated motion direction adaptation as well as form adaptation in response to dynamic as well as static distorted natural images. Two different psychophysical experiments were conducted, and adaptation aftereffects were estimated by the induced shift in perception (PSE) in the two features of form and motion. Each feature was assessed by a feature-typical test stimulus. In the first experiment, distortion-induced form adaptation was tested on skewed checkerboards, and in the second experiment, distortion-induced motion direction adaptation was tested on dynamic random dot patterns. Condition 1 confirmed the current literature on adaptation to skewed dynamic natural stimuli (Habtegiorgis, Jarvers, Rifai, Neumann, & Wahl, 2019b; Habtegiorgis, Rifai, Lappe, & Wahl, 2017c). But as a novelty, in Condition 2, both form adaptation and motion direction adaptation were induced in the absence of motion information. These results revealed that motion direction aftereffect and form aftereffect are induced even upon exposure to static distorted natural scenes. Particularly surprising, in Experiment 2, Condition 2, motion direction adaptation was shown in response to static distorted images. Thereby, an interaction of form and motion pathways in the adaptation process was revealed. 
Interaction between form and motion direction processing neurons exists on different levels in the hierarchy (Mather, Pavan, Bellacosa Marotti, Campana, & Casco, 2013). Direction-selective neurons extract motion orthogonal to preferred orientation already in V1 (De Valois, William Yund, & Hepler, 1982; Hubel & Wiesel, 1968). But evidence from psychophysics suggests that motion-form interactions also take place at a level of optic flow integration (Pavan, Marotti, & Mather, 2013). Even heading estimation has been shown to be impacted not only by motion but also by form features (Niehorster, Cheng, & Li, 2010). Thus, adaptation to static skew is likely to contribute to the changes in estimating motion direction (Fu et al., 2004; Mather et al., 2012). As natural image content contains lower- and higher-level features, one might even speculate on an interaction at the level of object perception (McGraw, Whitaker, Skillen, & Chung, 2002; Nishida & Johnston, 1999; Snowden, 1998) and that the interaction might be located in area MT where motion information is processed (McGraw, Walsh, & Barrett, 2004). 
Presbyopes wearing PALs for the first time often complain of distortion and image swim until they familiarize to PALs (Ahmad Najmee, Buari, Mujari, & Rahman, 2017; Pope, 2000; Shagufta & Rabia, 2017; Sullivan, 1990; Sullivan & Fowler, 1989). Thus, distortions show the potential to challenge daily life with PALs (e.g., during climbing stairs; Johnson, Buckley, Scally, & Elliott, 2007). 
The experimental paradigm demonstrated visual adaptation after exposure to image skew on a time scale of minutes and the specific role of the form feature. In a similar paradigm, adaptation aftereffects to skewed image content have been demonstrated to show long-term effects in the range of months after exposure of minutes, thus well connecting to the broad time range reported on PAL habituation (Ahmad Najmee et al., 2017; Habtegiorgis, Rifai, Lappe, & Wahl, 2018; Pope, 2000; Shagufta & Rabia, 2017). 
While the experimental paradigm focuses on foveal aftereffects, transsaccadic transfer of aftereffects demonstrated the nonfoveal occurrence of aftereffects in a comparable paradigm, as they might potentially contribute to PAL habituation (Habtegiorgis, Rifai, & Wahl, 2018). 
Here, adaptation of the features of form and motion in response to image skew was demonstrated together with an interaction of the two features. Specifically, a dominant role of form information was revealed, inducing form adaptation as well as motion direction adaptation. In sum, motion and form pathways contribute to the adaptation process to distortions and presumably interact during adaptation. For further studies on habituation in PALs, distorted images provide a useful abstraction level as adapting stimuli in experimental paradigms. Thus, the present study provides evidence that exposure to static image skew serves as an experimental paradigm for the induction and analysis of perceptual habituation to PALs. 
Acknowledgments
Commercial relationships: KW and SW are employees of Carl Zeiss Vision International GmbH. 
Corresponding author: Katharina Rifai. 
Email: katharina.rifai@medizin.uni-tuebingen.de. 
Address: Institute for Ophthalmic Research, University of Tübingen. 
References
Ahmad Najmee, N. A., Buari, N. H., Mujari, R., & Rahman, M. I. (2017). Satisfaction level of progressive additional lens (PALs) wearers. Environment-Behaviour Proceedings Journal, 2(6), 373, https://doi.org/10.21834/e-bpj.v2i6.999. [CrossRef]
Alvarez, T. L., Kim, E. H., & Granger-Donetti, B. (2017). Adaptation to progressive additive lenses: Potential factors to consider. Scientific Reports, 7(1), https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02851-5.
Barbero, S., & Portilla, J. (2015). Geometrical interpretation of dioptric blurring and magnification in ophthalmic lenses. Optics Express, 23(10), 13185, https://doi.org/10.1364/oe.23.013185. [CrossRef]
Barlow, H. B., & Olshausen, B. A. (2004). Convergent evidence for the visual analysis of optic flow through anisotropic attenuation of high spatial frequencies. Journal of Vision, 4(6), 415–426, https://doi.org/10.1167/4.6.1. [CrossRef]
Baumann, T. (2010). Valkaama.
Bex, P. J., & Makous, W. (2002). Spatial frequency, phase, and the contrast of natural images. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 19(6), 1096, https://doi.org/10.1364/josaa.19.001096. [CrossRef]
Bex, P. J., Mareschal, I., & Dakin, S. C. (2007). Contrast gain control in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 7(11), 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1167/7.11.12. [CrossRef]
Bex, P. J., Solomon, S. G., & Dakin, S. C. (2009). Contrast sensitivity in natural scenes depends on edge as well as spatial frequency structure. Journal of Vision, 9(10), https://doi.org/10.1167/9.10.1.
Billock, V. A., De Guzman, G. C., & Scott Kelso, J. A. (2001). Fractal time and 1/f spectra in dynamic images and human vision. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 148(1–2), 136–146, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2789(00)00174-3. [CrossRef]
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436, https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897x00357. [CrossRef]
Clifford, C. W. G., Webster, M. A., Stanley, G. B., Stocker, A. A., Kohn, A., Sharpee, T. O., & Schwartz, O. (2007). Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Research, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.023.
De Valois, R. L., William Yund, E., & Hepler, N. (1982). The orientation and direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision Research, 22(5), 531–544, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90112-2. [CrossRef]
Dong, D. W., & Atick, J. J. (1995). Statistics of natural time-varying images. Network: Computation in Neural Systems, 6(3), 345–358, https://doi.org/10.1088/0954-898X_6_3_003. [CrossRef]
Edwards, M., & Crane, M. F. (2007). Motion streaks improve motion detection. Vision Research, 47(6), 828–833, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.005. [CrossRef]
Epstein, W. (1972). Adaptation to uniocular image magnification: Is the underlying shift proprioceptive? Perception & Psychophysics, 11(1), 89–91, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212690. [CrossRef]
Fonda, G. (1980). Prescribing multifocal lenses. In Refraction and clinical optics (Vol. 10, p. 210).
Fu, Y. X., Shen, Y., Gao, H., & Dan, Y. (2004). Asymmetry in visual cortical circuits underlying motion-induced perceptual mislocalization. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(9), 2165–2171, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5145-03.2004. [CrossRef]
Geisler, W. S. (1999). Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction. Nature, 400(6739), 65–69, https://doi.org/10.1038/21886. [CrossRef]
Geisler, W. S., Albrecht, D. G., Crane, A. M., & Stern, L. (2001). Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. Visual Neuroscience, 18(4), 501–516, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523801184014. [CrossRef]
Gibson, J. J. (1937). Adaptation with negative after-effect. Psychological Review, 44(3), 222–244, https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061358. [CrossRef]
Gibson, J. J., & Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(5), 453–467, https://doi.org/10.1037/h0059826. [CrossRef]
Grossberg, S. (1991). Why do parallel cortical systems exist for the perception of static form and moving form? Perception & Psychophysics, 49(2), 117–141, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205033. [CrossRef]
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Jarvers, C., Rifai, K., Neumann, H., & Wahl, S. (2019a). The role of bottom-up and top-down cortical interactions in adaptation to natural scene statistics. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 13, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2019.00009.
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Jarvers, C., Rifai, K., Neumann, H., & Wahl, S. (2019b). The role of bottom-up and top-down cortical interactions in adaptation to natural scene statistics. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 13, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2019.00009.
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Rifai, K., Lappe, M., & Wahl, S. (2017a). Adaptation to skew distortions of natural scenes and retinal specificity of its aftereffects. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01158.
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Rifai, K., Lappe, M., & Wahl, S. (2017b). Adaptation to skew distortions of natural scenes and retinal specificity of its aftereffects. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1158. [CrossRef]
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Rifai, K., Lappe, M., & Wahl, S. (2017c). Adaptation to skew distortions of natural scenes and retinal specificity of its aftereffects. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01158.
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Rifai, K., Lappe, M., & Wahl, S. (2018). Experience-dependent long-term facilitation of skew adaptation. Journal of Vision, 18(9), 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/18.9.7. [CrossRef]
Habtegiorgis, S. W., Rifai, K., & Wahl, S. (2018). Transsaccadic transfer of distortion adaptation in a natural environment. Journal of Vision, 18(1), https://doi.org/10.1167/18.1.13.
Harris, F. J. (1978). On the use of Windows for harmonic analysis with the discrete Fourier transform. Proceedings of the IEEE, 66(1), 51–83, https://doi.org/10.1109/PROC.1978.10837. [CrossRef]
Heath, D. A., McCormack, G. L., & Vaughan, W. H. (1987). Mapping of ophthalmic lens distortions with a pinhole camera. Optometry and Vision Science, 64(10), 731–733, https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-198710000-00003. [CrossRef]
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 195(1), 215–243, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455. [CrossRef]
Johnson, L., Buckley, J. G., Scally, A. J., & Elliott, D. B. (2007). Multifocal spectacles increase variability in toe clearance and risk of tripping in the elderly. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48(4), 1466–1471, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.06-0586. [CrossRef]
Kinney, J. A., Luria, S. M., & Weitzman, D. O. (1968). Responses to the underwater distortions of visual stimuli (SMRL Report No. 541). U.S. Naval Submarine Medical Center, https://doi.org/10.21236/ad0678919.
Kluge, S. (2008). Die letzte Droge.
Kohler, I. (1962). Experiments with goggles. Scientific American, 206, 62–72, https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0562-62. [CrossRef]
Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience.
Mather, G., Pavan, A., Bellacosa Marotti, R., Campana, G., & Casco, C. (2013). Interactions between motion and form processing in the human visual system. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2013.00065.
Mather, G., Pavan, A., Bellacosa, R. M., & Casco, C. (2012). Psychophysical evidence for interactions between visual motion and form processing at the level of motion integrating receptive fields. Neuropsychologia, 50(1), 153–159, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.013. [CrossRef]
McGraw, P. V., Walsh, V., & Barrett, B. T. (2004). Motion-sensitive neurones in V5/MT modulate perceived spatial position. Current Biology, 14(12), 1090–1093, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.028. [CrossRef]
McGraw, P. V., Whitaker, D., Skillen, J., & Chung, S. T. L. (2002). Motion adaptation distorts perceived visual position. Current Biology, 12(23), 2042–2047, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01354-4. [CrossRef]
Meister, D. J., & Fisher, S. W. (2008). Progress in the spectacle correction of presbyopia. Part 1: Design and development of progressive lenses. In Clinical and experimental optometry. John Wiley, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1444-0938.2007.00245.x.
Niehorster, D. C., Cheng, J. C. K., & Li, L. (2010). Optimal combination of form and motion cues in human heading perception. Journal of Vision, 10(11), https://doi.org/10.1167/10.11.20.
Nishida, S., & Johnston, A. (1999). Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern. Nature, 397(6720), 610–612, https://doi.org/10.1038/17600. [CrossRef]
Olmos, A., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2004). A biologically inspired algorithm for the recovery of shading and reflectance images. Perception, 33(12), 1463–1473, https://doi.org/10.1068/p5321. [CrossRef]
Pavan, A., Marotti, R. B., & Mather, G. (2013). Motion-form interactions beyond the motion integration level: evidence for interactions between orientation and optic flow signals. Journal of Vision, 13(6), 16, https://doi.org/10.1167/13.6.16. [CrossRef]
Pick, H. L., & Hay, J. C. (1964). Adaptation to prismatic distortion. Psychonomic Science, 1(1–12), 199–200, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03342863. [CrossRef]
Pope, D. R. (2000). Progressive addition lenses: History, design, wearer satisfaction and trends. In OSA TOPS Vol. 35: Vision science and its applications (pp. 342–357). Washington, DC: OSA, https://doi.org/10.1364/VSIA.2000.NW9.
Roosendaal, T. (2012). Tears of steel.
Ross, H. (1979). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 347–348, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1979.tb01703.x.
Schütt, H. H., Harmeling, S., Macke, J. H., & Wichmann, F. A. (2016). Painfree and accurate Bayesian estimation of psychometric functions for (potentially) overdispersed data. Vision Research, 122, 105–123, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.02.002. [CrossRef]
Shagufta, P. M., & Rabia, . (2017). Adaptation period of different refractive prescriptions in pre presbyopic adults. Ophthalmology Pakistan, 7(2), 24–30. Retrieved from www.ophthalmologypakistan.com.
Sheedy, J. E., Campbell, C., King-Smith, E., & Hayes, J. R. (2005). Progressive powered lenses: The Minkwitz theorem. Optometry and Vision Science, 82(10), 916–922, https://doi.org/10.1097/01.opx.0000181266.60785.c9. [CrossRef]
Snowden, R. J. (1998). Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion. Current Biology, 8(24), 1343–1345, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(07)00567-2. [CrossRef]
Sullivan, C. M. (1990). Physical and psychophysical analysis of progressive addition lens embodiments.
Sullivan, C. M., & Fowler, C. W. (1989). Analysis of a progressive addition lens population. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 9(2), 163–170, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-1313.1989.tb00837.x. [CrossRef]
Vlaskamp, B. N. S., Filippini, H. R., & Banks, M. S. (2009). Image-size differences worsen stereopsis independent of eye position. Journal of Vision, 9(2), https://doi.org/10.1167/9.2.17.
Welch, R B. (1974). Research on adaptation to rearranged vision: 1966–1974. Perception, 3(4), 367–392, https://doi.org/10.1068/p030367. [CrossRef]
Welch, R. B., Carterette, E. C., & Friedman, M. P. (1978). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. Elsevier Science.
Figure 1.
 
(a) Example of a skewed natural image sequence used as adaptor in Condition 1 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Image skew is 25° and –25°, respectively. (b) Example of 25°/–25° skewed natural image used as adaptor in Condition 2 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Additional static images are shown in Supplementary Figure S1. (c) Example of plaid checkerboard test stimuli with left and right skew directions of –14.5% and 14.5%.
Figure 1.
 
(a) Example of a skewed natural image sequence used as adaptor in Condition 1 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Image skew is 25° and –25°, respectively. (b) Example of 25°/–25° skewed natural image used as adaptor in Condition 2 that is weighted by a Hanning window. Additional static images are shown in Supplementary Figure S1. (c) Example of plaid checkerboard test stimuli with left and right skew directions of –14.5% and 14.5%.
Figure 2.
 
FAE from all the observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows data, sigmoid fits, and confidence intervals at PSE in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Negative stimulus skew amplitudes refer to left-skewed stimuli, and positive skew amplitudes refer to right-skewed stimuli. The bar plots in the right column show the average ΔPSE of all observers. The error bars show the standard error.
Figure 2.
 
FAE from all the observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows data, sigmoid fits, and confidence intervals at PSE in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Negative stimulus skew amplitudes refer to left-skewed stimuli, and positive skew amplitudes refer to right-skewed stimuli. The bar plots in the right column show the average ΔPSE of all observers. The error bars show the standard error.
Figure 3.
 
Dynamic random dot test stimuli for measurement of motion direction aftereffects. Illustration of (a) upward and (b) downward motion direction.
Figure 3.
 
Dynamic random dot test stimuli for measurement of motion direction aftereffects. Illustration of (a) upward and (b) downward motion direction.
Figure 4.
 
MAE from all observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows psychometric curves of the average response of all the observers. Sigmoid fit, data, and confidence intervals at PSE are shown in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Positive θtilt corresponded to upward motion and negative to downward motion. In the second column, bar plots show the average ΔPSE of all the observers. The error bars show the standard error.
Figure 4.
 
MAE from all observers induced by dynamic skewed natural image sequences in Condition 1 (a) and by static skewed natural images in Condition 2 (b). The first column shows psychometric curves of the average response of all the observers. Sigmoid fit, data, and confidence intervals at PSE are shown in green for the left-skew adaptation aftereffects and in blue for the right-skew adaptation aftereffects. Positive θtilt corresponded to upward motion and negative to downward motion. In the second column, bar plots show the average ΔPSE of all the observers. The error bars show the standard error.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×