Although the results discussed so far appeared consistent with perceptual crowding, we did not observe a clear inwards-outwards asymmetry as is often reported for crowding (
Petrov et al., 2007; see
Figures 8,
9). One possible interpretation for this result is that the processes dominating our contextual modulation effect may not be the same as those in letter crowding. Nonetheless, another possible interpretation is that our contextual modulation phenomenon is produced by the same mechanisms as letter crowding, but that these mechanisms affect texture perception in our task differently from commonly used stimuli such as letters and vernier. For example, it has been shown that inward and outward flankers have different relative weight for different crowding processes and different crowding tasks (
Chastain, 1982;
Strasburger & Malania, 2013;
Strasburger, 2019), and also that classical inward-outward asymmetry can be reversed by biasing attention towards the fovea (
Petrov & Meleshkevich, 2011b). Therefore, it is possible that if the different subprocesses of crowding have different relative effects on textures than on letters, this may lead to a different overall inwards-outwards asymmetry. In line with this, we speculate that the large variability we observed across participants in the sensitivity to the different experimental conditions, such as surround position, reflects a variability in the relevance of the different underlying processes (whether the same as in classical crowding or not), which is also consistent with other crowding studies (
Kooi et al., 1994;
Petrov & Meleshkevich, 2011a;
Wallace et al., 2013;
Lev & Polat, 2015). Although our results do not allow to tell whether our contextual modulation phenomenon is different from letter crowding, or if it involves the same mechanisms as crowding but affecting textures differently, they point to the need of further studies on the relation between contextual modulation of textures and the phenomenon of crowding, which is frequently described as objects undergoing “forced texture processing” (
Rosenholtz, 2016). This would also be relevant, for example, to previous work studying crowding in natural scenes (
Wallis & Bex, 2012;
Gong et al., 2018), which according to this line of reasoning might also have measured, to an unknown degree, other contextual modulation processes affecting texture perception. As explained above, our work points to additional processes such as flexible surround suppression and facilitation whose relation to crowding is uncertain and which may be of particularly high relevance to texture contextual modulation. Finally, it is worth noting that some of the tasks most associated with peripheral vision such as scene perception (
Ehinger & Rosenholtz, 2016;
Brady, Shafer-Skelton, & Alvarez, 2017;
Groen, Silson, & Baker, 2017), guidance of eye movements (
Parkhurst & Niebur, 2004;
Frey, König, & Einhäuser, 2007;
Schmid & Victor, 2014) and the control of body movement (
Brandt, Dichgans, & Koenig, 1973;
Bardy, Warren, & Kay, 1999;
Berencsi, Ishihara, & Imanaka, 2005) have been proposed to use texture as a major source of information (
Harrington et al., 1985;
Sinai, Krebs, Darken, Rowland, & McCarley, 1999;
Parkhurst & Niebur, 2004;
Frey et al., 2007;
Schmid & Victor, 2014;
Brady et al., 2017;
Groen et al., 2017;
Ehinger & Rosenholtz, 2016). Therefore, understanding the role of contextual modulation on texture perception in the periphery may be an important step for understanding of the limitations of peripheral vision in natural behavior.