It is known that salient stimuli can temporarily deviate visuo-spatial attention even when participants are explicitly asked to ignore them and/or to pay attention to another location. This phenomenon is thought to be mainly related to an involuntary response to low-level stimulus features, such as luminance transients, which would imply a prominent role played by bottom-up processes (
Yantis & Jonides, 1984;
Yantis & Jonides, 1990). However, some evidence revealed a more active role of top-down control in mediating visuo-spatial attentional shifts by showing that a cue stimulus presented prior to the target is capable of attracting attention only if it contains overlapping properties with the target stimulus (
Anderson & Folk, 2010;
Folk, Leber, & Egeth, 2002;
Folk, Remington, & Wright, 1994;
Theeuwes, Olivers, & Belopolsky, 2010). Following this evidence, in
Experiment 2, we aimed to further understand if the attentional-related component of numerosity adaptation exhibited in
Experiment 1 was generally linked to the mere presence of an additional stimulus or, rather, reflected a numerosity-contingent visuo-spatial attention shift. To this aim, we used the same paradigm employed in
Experiment 1 while replacing the neutral adaptor with a two bars stimulus, which had the same aggregate area of the neutral adaptor. Interestingly, the presentation of a non-numerical adaptor on the reference location did not significantly change the magnitude of adaptation aftereffect relative to the condition in which a single lateralized adaptor was displayed. In other words, presenting a single numerical adaptor or presenting a numerical adaptor together with a non-numerical stimulus (i.e. bars) in a diametrical opposite position, produced comparable results in terms of adaptation magnitude. This evidence suggests that the reduced adaptation magnitude reported in
Experiment 1 was mainly numerosity-contingent. This result might be accounted for in, at least, two different ways. A first possibility is that numerosity, independently from the task that participants are performing, has a relative higher saliency compared with bars length perception and thus dragging more attention. This is in line with recent studies showing that numerosity is spontaneously perceived and has higher saliency compared to other non-numerical features (
Anobile, et al., 2019;
Cicchini, Anobile, & Burr, 2016;
Cicchini, Anobile, & Burr, 2019;
Ferrigno, Jara-Ettinger, Piantadosi, & Cantlon, 2017). A second possibility is that, given participants were engaged in a numerosity task, the numerical adaptor was more effective in dragging attentional resources because it was relatively more task-relevant compared to the bars adaptor (
Anderson & Folk, 2010;
Folk et al., 2002;
Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992;
Theeuwes et al., 2010). Whichever of these hypotheses is correct, we reported here the first evidence suggesting that numerosity adaptation partly depends on visuo-spatial attentional mechanisms specifically tuned to numerical quantities. These results are in line with a previous experiment showing a very weak effect of a non-numerical attentional manipulation on numerosity adaptation magnitude when numerical stimuli fall outside the subitizing range (
Burr, Anobile, & Turi, 2011). Differently to the current work, the authors manipulated the attentive resources through a double non-numerical task (color-orientation conjunction task), which was performed during the presentation of the stimuli that was to be compared. Results showed a similar adaptation effect between the single task conditions (where the distracting task was not performed) and the dual-task condition. Despite the methodological differences, these results are in line with results from
Experiment 2 of the present work revealing the lack of a change in adaptation magnitude when a non-numerical stimulus is used to modulate visuo-spatial attentional resources allocation. An interesting aspect of the study of Burr and collaborators is that the same attentional manipulation had a very strong effect for very low numbers (i.e. subitizing). In that case, numerosity adaptation was present only in the dual task condition. Very low numerosities (usually up to four) are known to be processed by the subitizing system, a mechanism that makes the performance errorless and very fast compared with higher numerosities (
Kaufman & Lord, 1949). The literature on the subitizing phenomenon reveals that it reflects a highly demanding attentional mechanism strongly suffering attentional deprivation across different sensory modalities and different dual-tasks paradigms (
Anobile et al., 2020;
Anobile et al., 2012;
Burr, Turi, & Anobile, 2010;
Olivers & Watson, 2008;
Pomè et al., 2019;
Vetter, Butterworth, & Bahrami, 2008). The attentive nature of such mechanisms likely explains why even a non-numerical attentive manipulation led to a strong effect in the adaptation magnitude for numerosity within the subitizing range.